Neuroscience of sleep

From HandWiki - Reading time: 45 min

Short description: Study of the neuroscientific and physiological basis of the nature of sleep
Sleeping Princess: An early 20th-century painting by Victor Vasnetsov

The neuroscience of sleep is the study of the neuroscientific and physiological basis of the nature of sleep and its functions. Traditionally, sleep has been studied as part of psychology and medicine.[1] The study of sleep from a neuroscience perspective grew to prominence with advances in technology and the proliferation of neuroscience research from the second half of the twentieth century.

The importance of sleep is demonstrated by the fact that organisms daily spend hours of their time in sleep, and that sleep deprivation can have disastrous effects ultimately leading to death in animals.[2][3] For a phenomenon so important, the purposes and mechanisms of sleep are only partially understood, so much so that as recently as the late 1990s[4] it was quipped: "The only known function of sleep is to cure sleepiness".[5] However, the development of improved imaging techniques like EEG, PET and fMRI, along with high computational power have led to an increasingly greater understanding of the mechanisms underlying sleep.

The fundamental questions in the neuroscientific study of sleep are:

  1. What are the correlates of sleep i.e. what are the minimal set of events that could confirm that the organism is sleeping?
  2. How is sleep triggered and regulated by the brain and the nervous system?
  3. What happens in the brain during sleep?
  4. How can we understand sleep function based on physiological changes in the brain?
  5. What causes various sleep disorders and how can they be treated?[6]

Other areas of modern neuroscience sleep research include the evolution of sleep, sleep during development and aging, animal sleep, mechanism of effects of drugs on sleep, dreams and nightmares, and stages of arousal between sleep and wakefulness.[7]

Introduction

Rapid eye movement sleep (REM), non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM or non-REM), and waking represent the three major modes of consciousness, neural activity, and physiological regulation.[8] NREM sleep itself is divided into multiple stages – N1, N2 and N3. Sleep proceeds in 90-minute cycles of REM and NREM, the order normally being N1 → N2 → N3 → N2 → REM. As humans fall asleep, body activity slows down. Body temperature, heart rate, breathing rate, and energy use all decrease. Brain waves slow down. The excitatory neurotransmitter acetylcholine becomes less available in the brain.[9] Humans often maneuver to create a thermally friendly environment—for example, by curling up into a ball if cold. Reflexes remain fairly active.

REM sleep is considered closer to wakefulness and is characterized by rapid eye movement and muscle atonia. NREM is considered to be deep sleep (the deepest part of NREM is called slow wave sleep), and is characterized by lack of prominent eye movement, or muscle paralysis. Especially during non-REM sleep, the brain uses significantly less energy during sleep than it does in waking. In areas with reduced activity, the brain restores its supply of adenosine triphosphate (ATP), the molecule used for short-term storage and transport of energy.[10] (Since in quiet waking the brain is responsible for 20% of the body's energy use, this reduction has an independently noticeable impact on overall energy consumption.)[11] During slow-wave sleep, humans secrete bursts of growth hormone. All sleep, even during the day, is associated with the secretion of prolactin.[12]

According to the Hobson & McCarley activation-synthesis hypothesis, proposed in 1975–1977, the alternation between REM and non-REM can be explained in terms of cycling, reciprocally influential neurotransmitter systems.[13] Sleep timing is controlled by the circadian clock, and in humans, to some extent by willed behavior. The term circadian comes from the Latin circa, meaning "around" (or "approximately"), and diem or dies, meaning "day". The circadian clock refers to a biological mechanism that governs multiple biological processes causing them to display an endogenous, entrainable oscillation of about 24 hours. These rhythms have been widely observed in plants, animals, fungi and cyanobacteria.

Correlates of sleep

Main page: Biology:Sleep cycle

One of the important questions in sleep research is clearly defining the sleep state. This problem arises because sleep was traditionally defined as a state of consciousness and not as a physiological state,[14][15] thus there was no clear definition of what minimum set of events constitute sleep and distinguish it from other states of partial or no consciousness. The problem of making such a definition is complicated because it needs to include a variety of modes of sleep found across different species.

At a symptomatic level, sleep is characterized by lack of reactivity to sensory inputs, low motor output, diminished conscious awareness and rapid reversibility to wakefulness.[16] However, to translate these into a biological definition is difficult because no single pathway in the brain is responsible for the generation and regulation of sleep. One of the earliest proposals was to define sleep as the deactivation of the cerebral cortex and the thalamus[17] because of near lack of response to sensory inputs during sleep. However, this was invalidated because both regions are active in some phases of sleep. In fact, it appears that the thalamus is only deactivated in the sense of transmitting sensory information to the cortex.[18]

Some of the other observations about sleep included decrease of sympathetic activity and increase of parasympathetic activity in non-REM sleep, and increase of heart rate and blood pressure accompanied by decrease in homeostatic response and muscle tone during REM sleep.[19][20] However, these symptoms are not limited to sleep situations and do not map to specific physiological definitions.

More recently, the problem of definition has been addressed by observing overall brain activity in the form of characteristic EEG patterns.[21] Each stage of sleep and wakefulness has a characteristic pattern of EEG which can be used to identify the stage of sleep. Waking is usually characterized by beta (12–30 Hz) and gamma (25–100 Hz) depending on whether there was a peaceful or stressful activity.[22] The onset of sleep involves slowing down of this frequency to the drowsiness of alpha (8–12 Hz) and finally to theta (4–10 Hz) of Stage 1 NREM sleep.[23] This frequency further decreases progressively through the higher stages of NREM and REM sleep. On the other hand, the amplitude of sleep waves is lowest during wakefulness (10–30μV) and shows a progressive increase through the various stages of sleep. Stage 2 is characterized by sleep spindles (intermittent clusters of waves at sigma frequency i.e. 12–14 Hz) and K complexes (sharp upward deflection followed by slower downward deflection). Stage 3 sleep has more sleep spindles.[24][25][26] Stage 3 has very high amplitude delta waves (0–4 Hz) and is known as slow wave sleep.[27] REM sleep is characterized by low amplitude, mixed frequency waves. A sawtooth wave pattern is often present.[28]

Ontogeny and phylogeny of sleep

Animal Sleep: Sleeping white tiger

The questions of how sleep evolved in the animal kingdom and how it developed in humans are especially important because they might provide a clue to the functions and mechanisms of sleep respectively.

Sleep evolution

The evolution of different types of sleep patterns is influenced by a number of selective pressures, including body size, relative metabolic rate, predation, type and location of food sources, and immune function.[29][30][31][32] Sleep (especially deep SWS and REM) is tricky behavior because it steeply increases predation risk. This means that, for sleep to have evolved, the functions of sleep should have provided a substantial advantage over the risk it entails. In fact, studying sleep in different organisms shows how they have balanced this risk by evolving partial sleep mechanisms or by having protective habitats. Thus, studying the evolution of sleep might give a clue not only to the developmental aspects and mechanisms, but also to an adaptive justification for sleep.

One challenge studying sleep evolution is that adequate sleep information is known only for two phyla of animals- chordata and arthropoda.[7] With the available data, comparative studies have been used to determine how sleep might have evolved. One question that scientists try to answer through these studies is whether sleep evolved only once or multiple times. To understand this, they look at sleep patterns in different classes of animals whose evolutionary histories are fairly well-known and study their similarities and differences.

Humans possess both slow wave and REM sleep, in both phases both eyes are closed and both hemispheres of the brain involved. Sleep has also been recorded in mammals other than humans. One study showed that echidnas possess only slow wave sleep (non-REM). This seems to indicate that REM sleep appeared in evolution only after therians.[33] But this has later been contested by studies that claim that sleep in echidna combines both modes into a single sleeping state.[34] Other studies have shown a peculiar form of sleep in odontocetes (like dolphins and porpoises). This is called the unihemispherical slow wave sleep (USWS). At any time during this sleep mode, the EEG of one brain hemisphere indicates sleep while that of the other is equivalent to wakefulness. In some cases, the corresponding eye is open. This might allow the animal to reduce predator risk and sleep while swimming in water, though the animal may also be capable of sleeping at rest.[35][36]

The correlates of sleep found for mammals are valid for birds as well i.e. bird sleep is very similar to mammals and involves both SWS and REM sleep with similar features, including closure of both eyes, lowered muscle tone, etc.[37] However, the proportion of REM sleep in birds is much lower. Also, some birds can sleep with one eye open if there is high predation risk in the environment.[38][39] This gives rise to the possibility of sleep in flight; considering that sleep is very important and some bird species can fly for weeks continuously, this seems to be the obvious result. However, sleep in flight has not been recorded, and is so far unsupported by EEG data. Further research may explain whether birds sleep during flight or if there are other mechanisms which ensure their remaining healthy during long flights in the absence of sleep.

Unlike in birds, very few consistent features of sleep have been found among reptile species. The only common observation is that reptiles do not have REM sleep.[7]

Sleep in some invertebrates has also been extensively studied, e.g., sleep in fruitflies (Drosophila)[40] and honeybees.[41] Some of the mechanisms of sleep in these animals have been discovered while others remain quite obscure. The features defining sleep have been identified for the most part, and like mammals, this includes reduced reaction to sensory input, lack of motor response in the form of antennal immobility, etc.

The fact that both the forms of sleep are found in mammals and birds, but not in reptiles (which are considered to be an intermediate stage) indicates that sleep might have evolved separately in both. Substantiating this might be followed by further research on whether the EEG correlates of sleep are involved in its functions or if they are merely a feature. This might further help in understanding the role of sleep in long term plasticity.

According to Tsoukalas (2012), REM sleep is an evolutionary transformation of a well-known defensive mechanism, the tonic immobility reflex. This reflex, also known as animal hypnosis or death feigning, functions as the last line of defense against an attacking predator and consists of the total immobilization of the animal: the animal appears dead (cf. "playing possum"). The neurophysiology and phenomenology of this reaction show striking similarities to REM sleep, a fact which betrays a deep evolutionary kinship. For example, both reactions exhibit brainstem control, paralysis, sympathetic activation, and thermoregulatory changes. This theory integrates many earlier findings into a unified, and evolutionary well informed, framework.[42][43]

Sleep development and aging

The ontogeny of sleep is the study of sleep across different age groups of a species, particularly during development and aging. Among mammals, infants sleep the longest.[44] Human babies have 8 hours of REM sleep and 8 hours of NREM sleep on an average. The percentage of time spent on each mode of sleep varies greatly in the first few weeks of development and some studies have correlated this to the degree of precociality of the child.[45] Within a few months of postnatal development, there is a marked reduction in percentage of hours spent in REM sleep. By the time the child becomes an adult, he spends about 6–7 hours in NREM sleep and only about an hour in REM sleep.[46][47] This is true not only of humans, but of many animals dependent on their parents for food.[48] The observation that the percentage of REM sleep is very high in the first stages of development has led to the hypothesis that REM sleep might facilitate early brain development.[45] However, this theory has been contested by other studies.

Sleep behavior undergoes substantial changes during adolescence. Some of these changes may be societal in humans, but other changes are hormonal. Another important change is the decrease in the number of hours of sleep, as compared to childhood, which gradually becomes identical to an adult. It is also being speculated that homeostatic regulation mechanisms may be altered during adolescence. Apart from this, the effect of changing routines of adolescents on other behavior such as cognition and attention is yet to be studied.[49][50] Ohayon et al, for example, have stated that the decline in total sleep time from childhood to adolescence seems to be more associated with environmental factors rather than biological feature.[51]

In adulthood, the sleep architecture has been showing that the sleep latency and the time spent in NREM stages 1 and 2 may increase with aging, while the time spent in REM and SWS sleep seem to decrease.[51] These changes have been frequently associated with brain atrophy, cognitive impairment and neurodegenerative disorders in old age.[51][52][53][54][55][56] For instance, Backhaus et al have pointed out that a decline in declarative memory consolidation in midlife (in their experiment: 48 to 55 years old) is due to a lower amount of SWS, which might already start to decrease around age of 30 years old.[52] According to Mander et al, atrophy in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) gray matter is a predictor of disruption in slow activity during NREM sleep that may impair memory consolidation in older adults.[53] And sleep disturbances, such as excessive daytime sleepiness and nighttime insomnia, have been often referred as factor risk of progressive functional impairment in Alzheimer's disease (AD) or Parkinson's disease (PD).[54][56]

Therefore, sleep in aging is another equally important area of research. A common observation is that many older adults spend time awake in bed after sleep onset in an inability to fall asleep and experience marked decrease in sleep efficiency.[57] There may also be some changes in circadian rhythms.[58] Studies are ongoing about what causes these changes and how they may be reduced to ensure comfortable sleep of old adults.

Brain activity during sleep

Slow Wave Sleep
REM Sleep
EEG waveforms of brain activity during sleep
Hypnogram showing sleep cycles from midnight to morning.
Hypnogram showing sleep architecture from midnight to 6:30 am, with deep sleep early on. There is more REM (marked red) before waking. (Current hypnograms reflect the recent decision to combine NREM stages 3 and 4 into a single stage 3.)

Understanding the activity of different parts of the brain during sleep can give a clue to the functions of sleep. It has been observed that mental activity is present during all stages of sleep, though from different regions in the brain. So, contrary to popular understanding, the brain never completely shuts down during sleep. Also, sleep intensity of a particular region is homeostatically related to the corresponding amount of activity before sleeping.[59][60] The use of imaging modalities like PET, fMRI and MEG, combined with EEG recordings, gives a clue to which brain regions participate in creating the characteristic wave signals and what their functions might be.[61]

Historical development of the stages model

The stages of sleep were first described in 1937 by Alfred Lee Loomis and his coworkers, who separated the different electroencephalography (EEG) features of sleep into five levels (A to E), representing the spectrum from wakefulness to deep sleep.[62] In 1953, REM sleep was discovered as distinct, and thus William C. Dement and Nathaniel Kleitman reclassified sleep into four NREM stages and REM.[21] The staging criteria were standardized in 1968 by Allan Rechtschaffen and Anthony Kales in the "R&K sleep scoring manual."[63][64]

In the R&K standard, NREM sleep was divided into four stages, with slow-wave sleep comprising stages 3 and 4. In stage 3, delta waves made up less than 50% of the total wave patterns, while they made up more than 50% in stage 4. Furthermore, REM sleep was sometimes referred to as stage 5. In 2004, the AASM commissioned the AASM Visual Scoring Task Force to review the R&K scoring system. The review resulted in several changes, the most significant being the combination of stages 3 and 4 into Stage N3. The revised scoring was published in 2007 as The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events.[65] Arousals, respiratory, cardiac, and movement events were also added.[66][67]

NREM sleep activity

NREM sleep is characterized by decreased global and regional cerebral blood flow. It constitutes ~80% of all sleep in adult humans.[68] Initially, it was expected that the brainstem, which was implicated in arousal would be inactive, but this was later on found to have been due to low resolution of PET studies and it was shown that there is some slow wave activity in the brainstem as well. However, other parts of the brain, including the precuneus, basal forebrain and basal ganglia are deactivated during sleep. Many areas of the cortex are also inactive, but to different levels. For example, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is considered the least active area while the primary cortex, the least deactivated.[25][69]

NREM sleep is characterized by slow oscillations, spindles and delta waves. The slow oscillations have been shown to be from the cortex, as lesions in other parts of the brain do not affect them, but lesions in the cortex do.[70] The delta waves have been shown to be generated by recurrent connections within the cerebral cortex. During slow wave sleep, the cortex generates brief periods of activity and inactivity at 0.5-4 Hz, resulting in the generation of the delta waves of slow wave sleep. During this period, the thalamus stops relaying sensory information to the brain, however it continues to produce signals, such as spindle waves, that are sent to its cortical projections. Sleep spindles of slow wave sleep are generated as an interaction of the thalamic reticular nucleus with thalamic relay neurons.[71] The sleep spindles have been predicted to play a role in disconnecting the cortex from sensory input and allowing entry of calcium ions into cells, thus potentially playing a role in plasticity.[72][73]

NREM 1

NREM Stage 1 (N1 – light sleep, somnolence, drowsy sleep – 5–10% of total sleep in adults): This is a stage of sleep that usually occurs between sleep and wakefulness, and sometimes occurs between periods of deeper sleep and periods of REM. The muscles are active, and the eyes roll slowly, opening and closing moderately. The brain transitions from alpha waves having a frequency of 8–13 Hz (common in the awake state) to theta waves having a frequency of 4–7 Hz. Sudden twitches and hypnic jerks, also known as positive myoclonus, may be associated with the onset of sleep during N1. Some people may also experience hypnagogic hallucinations during this stage. During Non-REM1, humans lose some muscle tone and most conscious awareness of the external environment.

NREM 2

NREM Stage 2 (N2 – 45–55% of total sleep in adults[74]): In this stage, theta activity is observed and sleepers become gradually harder to awaken; the alpha waves of the previous stage are interrupted by abrupt activity called sleep spindles (or thalamocortical spindles) and K-complexes.[75] Sleep spindles range from 11 to 16 Hz (most commonly 12–14 Hz). During this stage, muscular activity as measured by EMG decreases, and conscious awareness of the external environment disappears.

NREM 3

Main page: Biology:Slow-wave sleep
30 seconds of N3 – deep sleep.

NREM Stage 3 (N3 – 15–25% of total sleep in adults): Formerly divided into stages 3 and 4, this stage is called slow-wave sleep (SWS) or deep sleep. SWS is initiated in the preoptic area and consists of delta activity, high amplitude waves at less than 3.5 Hz. The sleeper is less responsive to the environment; many environmental stimuli no longer produce any reactions. Slow-wave sleep is thought to be the most restful form of sleep, the phase which most relieves subjective feelings of sleepiness and restores the body.[76]

This stage is characterized by the presence of a minimum of 20% delta waves ranging from 0.5–2 Hz and having a peak-to-peak amplitude >75 μV. (EEG standards define delta waves to be from 0 to 4 Hz, but sleep standards in both the original R&K model (Allan Rechtschaffen and Anthony Kales in the "R&K sleep scoring manual."),[63][64] as well as the new 2007 AASM guidelines have a range of 0.5–2 Hz.) This is the stage in which parasomnias such as night terrors, nocturnal enuresis, sleepwalking, and somniloquy occur. Many illustrations and descriptions still show a stage N3 with 20–50% delta waves and a stage N4 with greater than 50% delta waves; these have been combined as stage N3.[74]

REM sleep activity

Main page: Biology:Rapid eye movement sleep
30 seconds of REM sleep. Eye movements highlighted by red box.

REM Stage (REM Sleep – 20–25% of total sleep in adults[77]): REM sleep is where most muscles are paralyzed, and heart rate, breathing and body temperature become unregulated. REM sleep is turned on by acetylcholine secretion and is inhibited by neurons that secrete monoamines including serotonin. REM is also referred to as paradoxical sleep because the sleeper, although exhibiting high-frequency EEG waves similar to a waking state, is harder to arouse than at any other sleep stage.[75] Vital signs indicate arousal and oxygen consumption by the brain is higher than when the sleeper is awake.[78] REM sleep is characterized by high global cerebral blood flow, comparable to wakefulness.[79] In fact, many areas in the cortex have been recorded to have more blood flow during REM sleep than even wakefulness- this includes the hippocampus, temporal-occipital areas, some parts of the cortex, and basal forebrain. The limbic and paralimbic system including the amygdala are other active regions during REM sleep.[79][80] Though the brain activity during REM sleep appears very similar to wakefulness, the main difference between REM and wakefulness is that, arousal in REM is more effectively inhibited. This, along with the virtual silence of monoaminergic neurons in the brain, may be said to characterize REM.[81]

A newborn baby spends 8 to 9 hours a day just in REM sleep. By the age of five or so, only slightly over two hours is spent in REM.[82] The function of REM sleep is uncertain but a lack of it impairs the ability to learn complex tasks. Functional paralysis from muscular atonia in REM may be necessary to protect organisms from self-damage through physically acting out scenes from the often-vivid dreams that occur during this stage.

In EEG recordings, REM sleep is characterized by high frequency, low amplitude activity and spontaneous occurrence of beta and gamma waves. The best candidates for generation of these fast frequency waves are fast rhythmic bursting neurons in corticothalamic circuits. Unlike in slow wave sleep, the fast frequency rhythms are synchronized over restricted areas in specific local circuits between thalamocortical and neocortical areas. These are said to be generated by cholinergic processes from brainstem structures.

Apart from this, the amygdala plays a role in REM sleep modulation, supporting the hypothesis that REM sleep allows internal information processing. The high amygdalar activity may also cause the emotional responses during dreams.[83] Similarly, the bizarreness of dreams may be due to the decreased activity of prefrontal regions, which are involved in integrating information as well as episodic memory.

Ponto-geniculo-occipital waves

REM sleep is also related to the firing of ponto-geniculo-occipital waves (also called phasic activity or PGO waves) and activity in the cholinergic ascending arousal system. PGO waves have been recorded in the lateral geniculate nucleus and occipital cortex during the pre-REM period and are thought to represent dream content. The greater signal-to-noise ratio in the LG cortical channel suggests that visual imagery in dreams may appear before full development of REM sleep, but this has not yet been confirmed. PGO waves may also play a role in development and structural maturation of brain, as well as long term potentiation in immature animals, based on the fact that there is high PGO activity during sleep in the developmental brain.[84][85]

Network reactivation

The other form of activity during sleep is reactivation. Some electrophysiological studies have shown that neuronal activity patterns found during a learning task before sleep are reactivated in the brain during sleep.[86] This, along with the coincidence of active areas with areas responsible for memory have led to the theory that sleep might have some memory consolidation functions. In this relation, some studies have shown that after a sequential motor task, the pre-motor and visual cortex areas involved are most active during REM sleep, but not during NREM. Similarly, the hippocampal areas involved in spatial learning tasks are reactivated in NREM sleep, but not in REM. Such studies suggest a role of sleep in consolidation of specific memory types. It is, however, still unclear whether other types of memory are also consolidated by these mechanisms.[87]

Hippocampal neocortical dialog

The hippocampal neocortical dialog refers to the very structured interactions during SWS between groups of neurons called ensembles in the hippocampus and neocortex.[88][89] Sharp wave patterns (SPW) dominate the hippocampus during SWS and neuron populations in the hippocampus participate in organized bursts during this phase. This is done in synchrony with state changes in the cortex (DOWN/UP state) and coordinated by the slow oscillations in cortex. These observations, coupled with the knowledge that the hippocampus plays a role in short to medium term memory whereas the cortex plays a role in long term memory, have led to the hypothesis that the hippocampal neocortical dialog might be a mechanism through which the hippocampus transfers information to the cortex. Thus, the hippocampal neocortical dialog is said to play a role in memory consolidation.[90]

Sleep regulation

Sleep regulation refers to the control of when an organism transitions between sleep and wakefulness.[91] The key questions here are to identify which parts of the brain are involved in sleep onset and what their mechanisms of action are.[92] In humans and most animals sleep and wakefulness seems to follow an electronic flip-flop model i.e. both states are stable, but the intermediate states are not.[93][94] Of course, unlike in the flip-flop, in the case of sleep, there seems to be a timer ticking away from the minute of waking so that after a certain period one must sleep, and in such a case even waking becomes an unstable state. The reverse may also be true to a lesser extent.

Sleep onset

Main page: Biology:Sleep onset

Some light was thrown on the mechanisms on sleep onset by the discovery that lesions in the preoptic area and anterior hypothalamus lead to insomnia while those in the posterior hypothalamus lead to sleepiness.[95][96] This was further narrowed down to show that the central midbrain tegmentum is the region that plays a role in cortical activation. Thus, sleep onset seems to arise from activation of the anterior hypothalamus along with inhibition of the posterior regions and the central midbrain tegmentum. Further research has shown that the hypothalamic region called ventrolateral preoptic nucleus produces the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA that inhibits the arousal system during sleep onset.[93]

Models of sleep regulation

Sleep is regulated by two parallel mechanisms, homeostatic regulation and circadian regulation, controlled by the hypothalamus and the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), respectively. Although the exact nature of sleep drive is unknown, homeostatic pressure builds up during wakefulness and this continues until the person goes to sleep. Adenosine is thought to play a critical role in this and many people have proposed that the pressure build-up is partially due to adenosine accumulation. However, some researchers have shown that accumulation alone does not explain this phenomenon completely. The circadian rhythm is a 24-hour cycle in the body, which has been shown to continue even in the absence of environmental cues. This is caused by projections from the SCN to the brain stem.

This two process model was first proposed in 1982 by Borbely,[97] who called them Process S (homeostatic) and Process C (Circadian) respectively. He showed how the slow wave density increases through the night and then drops off at the beginning of the day while the circadian rhythm is like a sinusoid. He proposed that the pressure to sleep was the maximum when the difference between the two was highest.

In 1993, a different model called the opponent process model[98] was proposed. This model explained that these two processes opposed each other to produce sleep, as against Borbely's model. According to this model, the SCN, which is involved in the circadian rhythm, enhances wakefulness and opposes the homeostatic rhythm. In opposition is the homeostatic rhythm, regulated via a complex multisynaptic pathway in the hypothalamus that acts like a switch and shuts off the arousal system. Both effects together produce a see-saw like effect of sleep and wakefulness.[81] More recently, it has been proposed that both models have some validity to them, while new theories hold that inhibition of NREM sleep by REM could also play a role.[99] In any case, the two process mechanism adds flexibility to the simple circadian rhythm and could have evolved as an adaptive measure.

Thalamic regulation

Much of the brain activity in sleep has been attributed to the thalamus and it appears that the thalamus may play a critical role in SWS. The two primary oscillations in slow wave sleep, delta and the slow oscillation, can be generated by both the thalamus and the cortex. However, sleep spindles can only be generated by the thalamus, making its role very important. The thalamic pacemaker hypothesis[100] holds that these oscillations are generated by the thalamus but the synchronization of several groups of thalamic neurons firing simultaneously depends on the thalamic interaction with the cortex. The thalamus also plays a critical role in sleep onset when it changes from tonic to phasic mode, thus acting like a mirror for both central and decentral elements and linking distant parts of the cortex to co-ordinate their activity.[101][102][103]

Ascending reticular activating system

The ascending reticular activating system consists of a set of neural subsystems that project from various thalamic nuclei and a number of dopaminergic, noradrenergic, serotonergic, histaminergic, cholinergic, and glutamatergic brain nuclei.[104][105][106][107] When awake, it receives all kinds of non-specific sensory information and relays them to the cortex. It also modulates fight or flight responses and is hence linked to the motor system. During sleep onset, it acts via two pathways: a cholinergic pathway that projects to the cortex via the thalamus and a set of monoaminergic pathways that projects to the cortex via the hypothalamus. During NREM sleep this system is inhibited by GABAergic neurons in the ventrolateral preoptic area and parafacial zone, as well as other sleep-promoting neurons in distinct brain regions.

Sleep function

Sleep deprivation studies show that sleep is particularly important to normal brain function. Sleep is needed to remove reactive oxygen species caused by oxidative stress (and generally autophagy) and to repair DNA. REM sleep also decrease concentration of noradrenaline, which when in excess amount causes the cell to undergo apoptosis.

It is likely that sleep evolved to fulfill some primeval function and took on multiple functions over time[108] (analogous to the larynx, which controls the passage of food and air, but descended over time to develop speech capabilities).

The multiple hypotheses proposed to explain the function of sleep reflect the incomplete understanding of the subject. While some functions of sleep are known, others have been proposed but not completely substantiated or understood. Some of the early ideas about sleep function were based on the fact that most (if not all) external activity is stopped during sleep. Initially, it was thought that sleep was simply a mechanism for the body to "take a break" and reduce wear. Later observations of the low metabolic rates in the brain during sleep seemed to indicate some metabolic functions of sleep.[109] This theory is not fully adequate as sleep only decreases metabolism by about 5–10%.[110][111] With the development of EEG, it was found that the brain has almost continuous internal activity during sleep, leading to the idea that the function could be that of reorganization or specification of neuronal circuits or strengthening of connections.[112][113] These hypotheses are still being explored. Other proposed functions of sleep include- maintaining hormonal balance, temperature regulation and maintaining heart rate.

According to a recent sleep disruption and insomnia review study,[114] there are short-term and long-term negative consequences on healthy individuals. The short term consequences include increased stress responsivity and psychosocial issues such as impaired cognitive or academic performance and depression. Experiments indicated that, in healthy children and adults, episodes of fragmented sleep or insomnia increased sympathetic activation, which can disrupt mood and cognition. The long term consequences include metabolic issues such as glucose homeostasis disruption and even tumor formation and increased risks of cancer.

Preservation

The "Preservation and Protection" theory holds that sleep serves an adaptive function. It protects the animal during that portion of the 24-hour day in which being awake, and hence roaming around, would place the individual at greatest risk.[115] Organisms do not require 24 hours to feed themselves and meet other necessities. From this perspective of adaptation, organisms are safer by staying out of harm's way, where potentially they could be prey to other, stronger organisms. They sleep at times that maximize their safety, given their physical capacities and their habitats.

This theory fails to explain why the brain disengages from the external environment during normal sleep. However, the brain consumes a large proportion of the body's energy at any one time and preservation of energy could only occur by limiting its sensory inputs. Another argument against the theory is that sleep is not simply a passive consequence of removing the animal from the environment, but is a "drive"; animals alter their behaviors in order to obtain sleep.

Therefore, circadian regulation is more than sufficient to explain periods of activity and quiescence that are adaptive to an organism, but the more peculiar specializations of sleep probably serve different and unknown functions. Moreover, the preservation theory needs to explain why carnivores like lions, which are on top of the food chain and thus have little to fear, sleep the most. It has been suggested that they need to minimize energy expenditure when not hunting.

Waste clearance from the brain

During sleep, metabolic waste products, such as immunoglobulins, protein fragments or intact proteins like beta-amyloid, may be cleared from the interstitium via a glymphatic system of lymph-like channels coursing along perivascular spaces and the astrocyte network of the brain.[116][117][118] According to this model, hollow tubes between the blood vessels and astrocytes act like a spillway allowing drainage of cerebrospinal fluid carrying wastes out of the brain into systemic blood.[117][118] Such mechanisms, which remain under preliminary research as of 2017, indicate potential ways in which sleep is a regulated maintenance period for brain immune functions and clearance of beta-amyloid, a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease.[117]

Restoration

Wound healing has been shown to be affected by sleep.[119]

It has been shown that sleep deprivation affects the immune system.[120] It is now possible to state that "sleep loss impairs immune function and immune challenge alters sleep," and it has been suggested that sleep increases white blood cell counts.[121] A 2014 study found that depriving mice of sleep increased cancer growth and dampened the immune system's ability to control cancers.[122]

The effect of sleep duration on somatic growth is not completely known. One study recorded growth, height, and weight, as correlated to parent-reported time in bed in 305 children over a period of nine years (age 1–10). It was found that "the variation of sleep duration among children does not seem to have an effect on growth."[123] It is well established that slow-wave sleep affects growth hormone levels in adult men.[12] During eight hours' sleep, Van Cauter, Leproult, and Plat found that the men with a high percentage of SWS (average 24%) also had high growth hormone secretion, while subjects with a low percentage of SWS (average 9%) had low growth hormone secretion.[46]

There is some supporting evidence of the restorative function of sleep. The sleeping brain has been shown to remove metabolic waste products at a faster rate than during an awake state.[124] While awake, metabolism generates reactive oxygen species, which are damaging to cells. In sleep, metabolic rates decrease and reactive oxygen species generation is reduced allowing restorative processes to take over. It is theorized that sleep helps facilitate the synthesis of molecules that help repair and protect the brain from these harmful elements generated during waking.[125] The metabolic phase during sleep is anabolic; anabolic hormones such as growth hormones (as mentioned above) are secreted preferentially during sleep.

Energy conservation could as well have been accomplished by resting quiescent without shutting off the organism from the environment, potentially a dangerous situation. A sedentary nonsleeping animal is more likely to survive predators, while still preserving energy. Sleep, therefore, seems to serve another purpose, or other purposes, than simply conserving energy. Another potential purpose for sleep could be to restore signal strength in synapses that are activated while awake to a "baseline" level, weakening unnecessary connections that to better facilitate learning and memory functions again the next day; this means the brain is forgetting some of the things we learn each day.[126]

Endocrine function

The secretion of many hormones is affected by sleep-wake cycles. For example, melatonin, a hormonal timekeeper, is considered a strongly circadian hormone, whose secretion increases at dim light and peaks during nocturnal sleep, diminishing with bright light to the eyes.[127] In some organisms melatonin secretion depends on sleep, but in humans it is independent of sleep and depends only on light level. Of course, in humans as well as other animals, such a hormone may facilitate coordination of sleep onset. Similarly, cortisol and thyroid stimulating hormone (TSH) are strongly circadian and diurnal hormones, mostly independent of sleep.[128] In contrast, other hormones like growth hormone (GH) & prolactin are critically sleep-dependent, and are suppressed in the absence of sleep.[129] GH has maximum increase during SWS while prolactin is secreted early after sleep onset and rises through the night. In some hormones whose secretion is controlled by light level, sleep seems to increase secretion. Almost in all cases, sleep deprivation has detrimental effects. For example, cortisol, which is essential for metabolism (it is so important that animals can die within a week of its deficiency) and affects the ability to withstand noxious stimuli, is increased by waking and during REM sleep.[130] Similarly, TSH increases during nocturnal sleep and decreases with prolonged periods of reduced sleep, but increases during total acute sleep deprivation.[131][132]
Because hormones play a major role in energy balance and metabolism, and sleep plays a critical role in the timing and amplitude of their secretion, sleep has a sizable effect on metabolism. This could explain some of the early theories of sleep function that predicted that sleep has a metabolic regulation role.

Memory processing

Main page: Biology:Sleep and memory

According to Plihal & Born, sleep generally increases recalling of previous learning and experiences, and its benefit depends on the phase of sleep and the type of memory.[133] For example, studies based on declarative and procedural memory tasks applied over early and late nocturnal sleep, as well as wakefulness controlled conditions, have been shown that declarative memory improves more during early sleep (dominated by SWS) while procedural memory during late sleep (dominated by REM sleep).[134][135]

Regarding to declarative memory, the functional role of SWS has been associated with hippocampal replays of previously encoded neural patterns that seem to facilitate long-term memories consolidation.[134][135] This assumption is based on the active system consolidation hypothesis, which states that repeated reactivations of newly encoded information in hippocampus during slow oscillations in NREM sleep mediate the stabilization and gradually integration of declarative memory with pre-existing knowledge networks on the cortical level.[136] It assumes the hippocampus might hold information only temporarily and in fast-learning rate, whereas the neocortex is related to long-term storage and slow-learning rate.[134][135][137][138][139] This dialogue between hippocampus and neocortex occurs in parallel with hippocampal sharp-wave ripples and thalamo-cortical spindles, synchrony that drives the formation of spindle-ripple event which seems to be a prerequisite for the formation of long-term memories.[135][137][139][140]

Reactivation of memory also occurs during wakefulness and its function is associated with serving to update the reactivated memory with new encoded information, whereas reactivations during SWS are presented as crucial for memory stabilization.[135] Based on targeted memory reactivation (TMR) experiments that use associated memory cues to triggering memory traces during sleep, several studies have been reassuring the importance of nocturnal reactivations for the formation of persistent memories in neocortical networks, as well as highlighting the possibility of increasing people's memory performance at declarative recalls.[134][140][138][139][141]

Furthermore, nocturnal reactivation seems to share the same neural oscillatory patterns as reactivation during wakefulness, processes which might be coordinated by theta activity.[142] During wakefulness, theta oscillations have been often related to successful performance in memory tasks, and cued memory reactivations during sleep have been showing that theta activity is significantly stronger in subsequent recognition of cued stimuli as compared to uncued ones, possibly indicating a strengthening of memory traces and lexical integration by cuing during sleep.[143] However, the beneficial effect of TMR for memory consolidation seems to occur only if the cued memories can be related to prior knowledge.[144]

Other studies have been also looking at the specific effects of different stages of sleep on different types of memory. For example, it has been found that sleep deprivation does not significantly affect recognition of faces, but can produce a significant impairment of temporal memory (discriminating which face belonged to which set shown). Sleep deprivation was also found to increase beliefs of being correct, especially if they were wrong. Another study reported that the performance on free recall of a list of nouns is significantly worse when sleep deprived (an average of 2.8 ± 2 words) compared to having a normal night of sleep (4.7 ± 4 words). These results reinforce the role of sleep on declarative memory formation. This has been further confirmed by observations of low metabolic activity in the prefrontal cortex and temporal and parietal lobes for the temporal learning and verbal learning tasks respectively. Data analysis has also shown that the neural assemblies during SWS correlated significantly more with templates than during waking hours or REM sleep. Also, post-learning, post-SWS reverberations lasted 48 hours, much longer than the duration of novel object learning (1 hour), indicating long term potentiation.

Moreover, observations include the importance of napping: improved performance in some kinds of tasks after a 1-hour afternoon nap; studies of performance of shift workers, showing that an equal number of hours of sleep in the day is not the same as in the night. Current research studies look at the molecular and physiological basis of memory consolidation during sleep. These, along with studies of genes that may play a role in this phenomenon, together promise to give a more complete picture of the role of sleep in memory.

Renormalizing the synaptic strength

Sleep can also serve to weaken synaptic connections that were acquired over the course of the day but which are not essential to optimal functioning. In doing so, the resource demands can be lessened, since the upkeep and strengthening of synaptic connections constitutes a large portion of energy consumption by the brain and tax other cellular mechanisms such as protein synthesis for new channels.[126][145] Without a mechanism like this taking place during sleep, the metabolic needs of the brain would increase over repeated exposure to daily synaptic strengthening, up to a point where the strains become excessive or untenable.

Behavior change with sleep deprivation

One approach to understanding the role of sleep is to study the deprivation of it.[146] Sleep deprivation is common and sometimes even necessary in modern societies because of occupational and domestic reasons like round-the-clock service, security or media coverage, cross-time-zone projects etc. This makes understanding the effects of sleep deprivation very important.

Many studies have been done from the early 1900s to document the effect of sleep deprivation. The study of REM deprivation began with William C. Dement around 1960. He conducted a sleep and dream research project on eight subjects, all male. For a span of up to 7 days, he deprived the participants of REM sleep by waking them each time they started to enter the stage. He monitored this with small electrodes attached to their scalp and temples. As the study went on, he noticed that the more he deprived the men of REM sleep, the more often he had to wake them. Afterwards, they showed more REM sleep than usual, later named REM rebound.[147][148]

The neurobehavioral basis for these has been studied only recently. Sleep deprivation has been strongly correlated with increased probability of accidents and industrial errors.[149][150] Many studies have shown the slowing of metabolic activity in the brain with many hours of sleep debt.[131] Some studies have also shown that the attention network in the brain is particularly affected by lack of sleep,[151] and though some of the effects on attention may be masked by alternate activities (like standing or walking) or caffeine consumption,[152] attention deficit cannot be completely avoided.

Sleep deprivation has been shown to have a detrimental effect on cognitive tasks, especially involving divergent functions or multitasking.[149][153][154] It also has effects on mood and emotion, and there have been multiple reports of increased tendency for rage, fear or depression with sleep debt.[155][156] However, some of the higher cognitive functions seem to remain unaffected albeit slower.[153] Many of these effects vary from person to person[157] i.e. while some individuals have high degrees of cognitive impairment with lack of sleep, in others, it has minimal effects. The exact mechanisms for the above are still unknown and the exact neural pathways and cellular mechanisms of sleep debt are still being researched.

Sleep disorders

Main page: Medicine:Sleep disorder

A sleep disorder, or somnipathy, is a medical disorder of the sleep patterns of a person or animal. Polysomnography is a test commonly used for diagnosing some sleep disorders. Sleep disorders are broadly classified into dyssomnias, parasomnias, circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD), and other disorders including ones caused by medical or psychological conditions and sleeping sickness. Some common sleep disorders include insomnia (chronic inability to sleep), sleep apnea (abnormally low breathing during sleep), narcolepsy (excessive sleepiness at inappropriate times), cataplexy (sudden and transient loss of muscle tone), and sleeping sickness (disruption of sleep cycle due to infection). Other disorders that are being studied include sleepwalking, sleep terror and bed wetting.

Studying sleep disorders is particularly useful as it gives some clues as to which parts of the brain may be involved in the modified function. This is done by comparing the imaging and histological patterns in normal and affected subjects. Treatment of sleep disorders typically involves behavioral and psychotherapeutic methods though other techniques may also be used. The choice of treatment methodology for a specific patient depends on the patient's diagnosis, medical and psychiatric history, and preferences, as well as the expertise of the treating clinician. Often, behavioral or psychotherapeutic and pharmacological approaches are compatible and can effectively be combined to maximize therapeutic benefits.

Frequently, sleep disorders have been also associated with neurodegenerative diseases, mainly when they are characterized by abnormal accumulation of alpha-synuclein, such as multiple system atrophy (MSA), Parkinson's disease (PD) and Lewy body disease (LBD).[158][54][159][56][160][161][162] For instance, people diagnosed with PD have often presented different kinds of sleep concerns, commonly regard to insomnia (around 70% of the PD population), hypersomnia (more than 50% of the PD population), and REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD) - that may affect around 40% of the PD population and it is associated with increased motor symptoms.[158][54][159][56][160][162] Furthermore, RBD has been also highlighted as a strong precursor of future development of those neurodegenerative diseases over several years in prior, which seems to be a great opportunity for improving treatments.[54][56]

Sleep disturbances have been also observed in Alzheimer's disease (AD), affecting about 45% of its population.[54][56][161] Moreover, when it is based on caregiver reports this percentage is even higher, about 70%.[163] As well as in PD population, insomnia and hypersomnia are frequently recognized in AD patients, which are associated with accumulation of Beta-amyloid, circadian rhythm sleep disorders (CRSD) and melatonin alteration.[54][56][163] Additionally, changes in sleep architecture are observed in AD too.[54][56][161] Even though with ageing the sleep architecture seems to change naturally, in AD patients it is aggravated. SWS is potentially decreased (sometimes totally absent), spindles and the time spent in REM sleep are also reduced, while its latency is increased.[163] The poorly sleep onset in AD has been also associated with dream-related hallucination, increased restlessness, wandering and agitation, that seem to be related with sundowning - a typical chronobiological phenomenon presented in the disease.[56][163]

The neurodegenerative conditions are commonly related to brain structures impairments, which might disrupt the states of sleep and wakefulness, circadian rhythm, motor or non motor functioning.[54][56] On the other hand, sleep disturbances are also frequently related to worsening patient's cognitive functioning, emotional state and quality of life.[56][162][163] Furthermore, these abnormal behavioural symptoms negatively contribute to overwhelming their relatives and caregivers.[56][162][163] Therefore, a deeper understanding of the relationship between sleep disorders and neurodegenerative diseases seems to be extremely important, mainly considering the limited research related to it and the increasing expectancy of life.[54][163]

A related field is that of sleep medicine which involves the diagnosis and therapy of sleep disorders and sleep deprivation, which is a major cause of accidents. This involves a variety of diagnostic methods including polysomnography, sleep diary, multiple sleep latency test, etc. Similarly, treatment may be behavioral such as cognitive behavioral therapy or may include pharmacological medication or bright light therapy.

Dreaming

Main page: Biology:Dream
"The Knight's Dream", a 1655 painting by Antonio de Pereda

Dreams are successions of images, ideas, emotions, and sensations that occur involuntarily in the mind during certain stages of sleep (mainly the REM stage). The content and purpose of dreams are not yet clearly understood though various theories have been proposed. The scientific study of dreams is called oneirology.

There are many theories about the neurological basis of dreaming. This includes the activation synthesis theory—the theory that dreams result from brain stem activation during REM sleep; the continual activation theory—the theory that dreaming is a result of activation and synthesis but dreams and REM sleep are controlled by different structures in the brain; and dreams as excitations of long term memory—a theory which claims that long term memory excitations are prevalent during waking hours as well but are usually controlled and become apparent only during sleep.

There are multiple theories about dream function as well. Some studies claim that dreams strengthen semantic memories. This is based on the role of hippocampal neocortical dialog and general connections between sleep and memory. One study surmises that dreams erase junk data in the brain. Emotional adaptation and mood regulation are other proposed functions of dreaming.

From an evolutionary standpoint, dreams might simulate and rehearse threatening events, that were common in the organism's ancestral environment, hence increasing a person's ability to tackle everyday problems and challenges in the present. For this reason these threatening events may have been passed on in the form of genetic memories.[164][165] This theory accords well with the claim that REM sleep is an evolutionary transformation of a well-known defensive mechanism, the tonic immobility reflex.[42][43]

Most theories of dream function appear to be conflicting, but it is possible that many short-term dream functions could act together to achieve a bigger long-term function.[7] It may be noted that evidence for none of these theories is entirely conclusive.

The incorporation of waking memory events into dreams is another area of active research and some researchers have tried to link it to the declarative memory consolidation functions of dreaming.[166][167]

A related area of research is the neuroscience basis of nightmares. Many studies have confirmed a high prevalence of nightmares and some have correlated them with high stress levels.[168] Multiple models of nightmare production have been proposed including neo-Freudian models as well as other models such as image contextualization model, boundary thickness model, threat simulation model etc.[169] Neurotransmitter imbalance has been proposed as a cause of nightmares, as also affective network dysfunction- a model which claims that nightmare is a product of dysfunction of circuitry normally involved in dreaming.[170][171] As with dreaming, none of the models have yielded conclusive results and studies continue about these questions.

See also

References

  1. "A brief history of sleep research". http://www.stanford.edu/~dement/history.html. 
  2. "NCBI Sleep Guide". http://science.education.nih.gov/supplements/nih3/sleep/guide/info-sleep.htm. 
  3. "No evidence of brain cell degeneration after long-term sleep deprivation in rats". Brain Research 840 (1–2): 184–93. September 1999. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01768-0. PMID 10517970. 
  4. The Neuroscience of Sleep. Academic Press. 22 May 2010. p. xiii. ISBN 9780123757227. https://books.google.com/books?id=E15ToNvBLcQC&q=%22to+cure+sleepiness%22&pg=PR13. Retrieved 18 July 2015. 
  5. "The Work We Do While We Sleep". The New Yorker. 8 July 2015. http://www.newyorker.com/science/maria-konnikova/why-we-sleep. Retrieved 17 July 2015. "The Harvard sleep researcher Robert Stickgold has recalled his former collaborator J. Allan Hobson joking that the only known function of sleep is to cure sleepiness.". 
  6. "New developments in sleep research: molecular genetics, gene expression, and systems neurobiology". The Journal of Neuroscience 28 (46): 11814–8. November 2008. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3768-08.2008. PMID 19005045. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 The Neuroscience of Sleep. Amsterdam: Boston : Academic Press/Elsevier. 2009. pp. 61–86. ISBN 9780123750730. https://archive.org/details/neuroscienceslee00rsti. 
  8. J. Alan Hobson, Edward F. Pace-Scott, & Robert Stickgold (2000), "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states", Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23.
  9. Brown, pp. 1100–1102.
  10. Brown, pp. 1118–1119: "Compared with wakefulness, sleep reduces brain energy demands, as suggested by the 44% reduction in the cerebral metabolic rate (CMR) of glucose (791) and a 25% reduction in the CMR of O2 (774) during sleep."
  11. "Do all animals sleep?". Trends in Neurosciences 31 (4): 208–13. April 2008. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.001. PMID 18328577. 
  12. 12.0 12.1 Eve Van Cauter & Karine Spiegel (1999). "Circadian and Sleep Control of Hormonal Secretions", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 397–425.
  13. "The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process". The American Journal of Psychiatry 134 (12): 1335–48. December 1977. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. 
  14. "Online Free Medical Dictionary". http://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/sleep. 
  15. Sleep : a scientific perspective. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. 1988. ISBN 9780138129187. https://archive.org/details/sleepscientificp00anch. 
  16. The encyclopedia of sleep and sleep disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Facts on File. 2001. ISBN 978-0-8160-4089-6. 
  17. "Thalamic deactivation at sleep onset precedes that of the cerebral cortex in humans". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (8): 3829–33. February 2010. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909710107. PMID 20142493. Bibcode2010PNAS..107.3829M. 
  18. "Synchrony in the interconnected circuitry of the thalamus and cerebral cortex". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1157 (1): 10–23. March 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04534.x. PMID 19351352. Bibcode2009NYASA1157...10J. 
  19. "Sympathetic-nerve activity during sleep in normal subjects". The New England Journal of Medicine 328 (5): 303–7. February 1993. doi:10.1056/NEJM199302043280502. PMID 8419815. 
  20. "Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis". Sleep Medicine Reviews 10 (1): 49–62. February 2006. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002. PMID 16376591. 
  21. 21.0 21.1 "Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 9 (4): 673–90. November 1957. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(57)90088-3. PMID 13480240. 
  22. "Gamma, fast, and ultrafast waves of the brain: their relationships with epilepsy and behavior". Epilepsy & Behavior 13 (1): 25–31. July 2008. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.01.011. PMID 18439878. 
  23. "Brain wave patterns accompanying changes in sleep and wakefulness during hypnosis". Psychosomatic Medicine 10 (6): 317–26. Nov–Dec 1948. doi:10.1097/00006842-194811000-00002. PMID 18106841. 
  24. "Sleep spindles". Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 2 (1): 1–35. January 1985. doi:10.1097/00004691-198501000-00001. PMID 3932462. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 "Regional cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and spindle activity during slow wave sleep in humans". The Journal of Neuroscience 17 (12): 4800–8. June 1997. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-12-04800.1997. PMID 9169538. 
  26. "Discriminative responses to stimulation during human sleep". Brain 83 (3): 440–53. September 1960. doi:10.1093/brain/83.3.440. PMID 13731563. 
  27. "Distribution of disturbance-patterns in the human electroencephalogram with special reference to sleep". Journal of Neurophysiology 1 (5): 413–430. 1938. doi:10.1152/jn.1938.1.5.413. 
  28. "Spectral analysis of EEG activity in different sleep stages in normal adults". European Neurology 7 (5): 265–96. 1 January 1972. doi:10.1159/000114432. PMID 4339034. 
  29. McNamara, P., R. A. Barton, and C. L. Nunn. 2010, Evolution of sleep: Phylogenetic and functional perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  30. "Energetic constraints, not predation, influence the evolution of sleep patterning in mammals". Functional Ecology 22 (5): 847–853. October 2008. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01449.x. PMID 20428321. 
  31. "To sleep or not to sleep: the ecology of sleep in artificial organisms". BMC Ecology 8: 10. May 2008. doi:10.1186/1472-6785-8-10. PMID 18479523. 
  32. Preston, B. T., I. Capellini, P. McNamara, R. A. Barton, and C. L. Nunn. 2009. Parasite resistance and the adaptive significance of sleep. Bmc Evolutionary Biology 9.
  33. "Electrophysiological studies of the echidna, Tachyglossus aculeatus. I. Waking and sleep". Archives Italiennes de Biologie 110 (2): 145–84. July 1972. PMID 4342268. 
  34. "The echidna Tachyglossus aculeatus combines REM and non-REM aspects in a single sleep state: implications for the evolution of sleep". The Journal of Neuroscience 16 (10): 3500–6. May 1996. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03500.1996. PMID 8627382. 
  35. "Unihemispheric slow-wave sleep in the Amazonian dolphin, Inia geoffrensis". Neuroscience Letters 79 (1–2): 128–32. August 1987. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(87)90684-7. PMID 3670722. 
  36. "Unihemispheric slow wave sleep and the state of the eyes in a white whale". Behavioural Brain Research 129 (1–2): 125–9. February 2002. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00346-1. PMID 11809503. 
  37. "A phylogenetic analysis of the correlates of sleep in birds". Journal of Sleep Research 15 (4): 395–402. December 2006. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00559.x. PMID 17118096. 
  38. "Monocular sleep in male domestic chicks". Behavioural Brain Research 153 (2): 447–52. August 2004. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.12.022. PMID 15265641. 
  39. "Facultative control of avian unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predation". Behavioural Brain Research 105 (2): 163–72. November 1999. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00070-4. PMID 10563490. 
  40. "Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster". Science 287 (5459): 1834–7. March 2000. doi:10.1126/science.287.5459.1834. PMID 10710313. Bibcode2000Sci...287.1834S. 
  41. "The dynamics of sleep-like behaviour in honey bees". Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural & Behavioral Physiology 189 (8): 599–607. August 2003. doi:10.1007/s00359-003-0436-9. PMID 12861424. 
  42. 42.0 42.1 "The origin of REM sleep: A hypothesis". Dreaming 22 (4): 253–283. 2012. doi:10.1037/a0030790. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 "Exploring the Mystery of REM Sleep". Psychology Today On-line blog. 25 March 2013. http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/media-spotlight/201303/exploring-the-mystery-rem-sleep. 
  44. "Infant sleep patterns: From birth to 16 weeks of age". The Journal of Pediatrics 65 (4): 576–82. October 1964. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(64)80291-2. PMID 14216645. 
  45. 45.0 45.1 "Sleep-wake patterns in preterm infants and 6 month's home environment: implications for early cognitive development". Early Human Development 68 (2): 93–102. July 2002. doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00018-X. PMID 12113995. 
  46. 46.0 46.1 "Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men". JAMA 284 (7): 861–8. August 2000. doi:10.1001/jama.284.7.861. PMID 10938176. 
  47. "Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle". Science 152 (3722): 604–19. April 1966. doi:10.1126/science.152.3722.604. PMID 17779492. Bibcode1966Sci...152..604R. 
  48. "Ontogenesis of circadian sleep-wakefulness rhythms and developmental changes of sleep in the altricial rat and in the precocial guinea pig". Behavioural Brain Research 11 (3): 185–96. March 1984. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(84)90210-9. PMID 6721913. 
  49. "Adolescent sleep patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days". Sleep 21 (8): 871–81. December 1998. doi:10.1093/sleep/21.8.871. PMID 9871949. 
  50. "Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents". Child Development 69 (4): 875–87. August 1998. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06149.x. PMID 9768476. 
  51. 51.0 51.1 51.2 "Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: Developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan". Sleep 27 (7): 1255–73. 2004. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255. PMID 15586779. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 "Midlife Decline in Declarative Memory Consolidation Is Correlated with a Decline in Slow Wave Sleep". Learning & Memory 14 (5): 336–341. 2007. doi:10.1101/lm.470507. PMID 17522024. 
  53. 53.0 53.1 Mander, B., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J., Lindquist, J., Ancoli-Israel, S., . . . Walker, M. (2013). Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. 357 - 364.
  54. 54.0 54.1 54.2 54.3 54.4 54.5 54.6 54.7 54.8 54.9 "Sleep–wake disturbances in common neurodegenerative diseases: A closer look at selected aspects of the neural circuitry". Journal of the Neurological Sciences 307 (1–2): 9–14. 2011. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2011.04.020. PMID 21570695. 
  55. "Deepening sleep by hypnotic suggestion". Sleep 37 (6): 1143–52. 2014. doi:10.5665/sleep.3778. PMID 24882909. 
  56. 56.00 56.01 56.02 56.03 56.04 56.05 56.06 56.07 56.08 56.09 56.10 56.11 "Sleep in Neurodegenerative Disorders". Current Sleep Medicine Reports 1 (2): 81–90. 2015. doi:10.1007/s40675-015-0016-x. 
  57. "Delayed sleep phase syndrome. A chronobiological disorder with sleep-onset insomnia". Archives of General Psychiatry 38 (7): 737–46. July 1981. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780320017001. PMID 7247637. 
  58. "Changes in circadian rhythms and sleep quality with aging: mechanisms and interventions". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 19 (4): 553–71. 1995. doi:10.1016/0149-7634(95)00018-6. PMID 8684716. 
  59. "Processes underlying sleep regulation". Hormone Research 49 (3–4): 114–7. 1 January 1998. doi:10.1159/000023156. PMID 9550110. https://www.zora.uzh.ch/id/eprint/885/1/23156.pdf. 
  60. "Regional cerebral blood flow increases during wakeful rest following cognitive training". Brain Research Bulletin 80 (3): 133–8. September 2009. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.06.021. PMID 19589374. 
  61. "Cortical source localization of sleep-stage specific oscillatory activity". Scientific Reports 10 (1): 6976. April 2020. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-63933-5. PMID 32332806. Bibcode2020NatSR..10.6976B. 
  62. "III Cerebral states during sleep, as studied by human brain potentials". J. Exp. Psychol. 21 (2): 127–44. 1937. doi:10.1037/h0057431. 
  63. 63.0 63.1 Brown, pp. 1108–1109.
  64. 64.0 64.1 A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects.. Washington: Public Health Service, US Government Printing Office. 1968. http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Rechschaffen-Kales---Manual.pdf. 
  65. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2007. 
  66. "Stages of Sleep". Psychology World. 1998. http://web.mst.edu/~psyworld/general/sleepstages/sleepstages.pdf. "(includes illustrations of "sleep spindles" and "K-complexes")" 
  67. "Rethinking sleep analysis". Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 4 (2): 99–103. April 2008. doi:10.5664/jcsm.27124. PMID 18468306. 
  68. Parmeggiani (2011), Systemic Homeostasis and Poikilostasis in Sleep, passim.
  69. "Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2(15)O PET study". Brain 120 ( Pt 7) (7): 1173–97. July 1997. doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173. PMID 9236630. 
  70. "Sleep suppression after basal forebrain lesions in the cat". Science 160 (3833): 1253–5. June 1968. doi:10.1126/science.160.3833.1253. PMID 5689683. Bibcode1968Sci...160.1253M. 
  71. McCormick, DA; Bal, T (1997). "Sleep and Arousal: Thalamocortical mechanisms.". Annual Review of Neuroscience 20: 185–215. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.185. PMID 9056712. 
  72. "Synchronized activities of coupled oscillators in the cerebral cortex and thalamus at different levels of vigilance". Cerebral Cortex 7 (6): 583–604. September 1997. doi:10.1093/cercor/7.6.583. PMID 9276182. 
  73. "Spatiotemporal patterns of spindle oscillations in cortex and thalamus". The Journal of Neuroscience 17 (3): 1179–96. February 1997. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-03-01179.1997. PMID 8994070. 
  74. 74.0 74.1 "Neurobiology of the sleep-wake cycle: sleep architecture, circadian regulation, and regulatory feedback". Journal of Biological Rhythms 21 (6): 482–93. December 2006. doi:10.1177/0748730406294627. PMID 17107938. 
  75. 75.0 75.1 Psychology. Worth Publishers. 2009. ISBN 978-1-4292-0615-0. 
  76. "Daily rhythms of the sleep-wake cycle". Journal of Physiological Anthropology 31 (1): 5. March 2012. doi:10.1186/1880-6805-31-5. PMID 22738268. 
  77. Psychology, Seventh Edition, in Modules (High School Version). Macmillan. 22 September 2003. pp. 268–. ISBN 978-0-7167-8595-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=oYuBwPDsQZoC&pg=PA268. Retrieved 22 August 2012. 
  78. Anatomy and Physiology: The Unity of Form and Function, 6th Edition. McGraw-Hill. 2012. p. 537. ISBN 978-0-07-337825-1. 
  79. 79.0 79.1 "The effect of slow wave sleep and REM sleep on regional cerebral blood flow in cats". Journal of Neurochemistry 15 (4): 301–6. April 1968. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1968.tb11614.x. PMID 5641651. 
  80. "Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming". Nature 383 (6596): 163–6. September 1996. doi:10.1038/383163a0. PMID 8774879. Bibcode1996Natur.383..163M. http://www-psych.stanford.edu/~knutson/ans/maquet96.pdf. Retrieved 2012-07-28. 
  81. 81.0 81.1 "Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms". Nature 437 (7063): 1257–63. October 2005. doi:10.1038/nature04284. PMID 16251950. Bibcode2005Natur.437.1257S. 
  82. "Sleep". Encarta Encyclopedia. Microsoft. 1999. http://www.npi.ucla.edu/sleepresearch/encarta/Article.htm. Retrieved 25 January 2008. 
  83. "The stuff dreams are made of: anatomical substrates of REM sleep". Nature Neuroscience 9 (6): 721–2. June 2006. doi:10.1038/nn0606-721. PMID 16732200. 
  84. "REM sleep burst neurons, PGO waves, and eye movement information". Journal of Neurophysiology 50 (4): 784–97. October 1983. doi:10.1152/jn.1983.50.4.784. PMID 6631463. 
  85. "The neuropsychology of REM sleep dreaming". NeuroReport 9 (3): R1–14. February 1998. doi:10.1097/00001756-199802160-00033. PMID 9512371. 
  86. "Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep". Science 265 (5172): 676–9. July 1994. doi:10.1126/science.8036517. PMID 8036517. Bibcode1994Sci...265..676W. 
  87. "Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation". Sleep Medicine 8 (4): 331–43. June 2007. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.011. PMID 17470412. 
  88. "The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue". Cerebral Cortex 6 (2): 81–92. 1 January 1996. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.2.81. PMID 8670641. 
  89. "Memory consolidation during sleep: a neurophysiological perspective". Journal of Sleep Research 7 Suppl 1 (S1): 17–23. 1 June 1998. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.7.s1.3.x. PMID 9682189. 
  90. "Hippocampal sleep features: relations to human memory function". Frontiers in Neurology 3: 57. 1 January 2012. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00057. PMID 22529835. 
  91. "Harvard Medical School Sleep Basics". http://healthysleep.med.harvard.edu/healthy/science/how. 
  92. The Neural Control of Sleep and Waking. Springer. 2002. ISBN 9780387955360. https://archive.org/details/neuralcontrolofs0000sieg. 
  93. 93.0 93.1 "The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness". Trends in Neurosciences 24 (12): 726–31. December 2001. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(00)02002-6. PMID 11718878. 
  94. "A putative flip-flop switch for control of REM sleep". Nature 441 (7093): 589–94. June 2006. doi:10.1038/nature04767. PMID 16688184. Bibcode2006Natur.441..589L. 
  95. "Long-lasting insomnia induced by preoptic neuron lesions and its transient reversal by muscimol injection into the posterior hypothalamus in the cat". Neuroscience 32 (3): 669–83. 1989. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(89)90289-3. PMID 2601839. 
  96. "The effects of posterior hypothalamic lesions on behavioral and electrographic manifestations of sleep and waking in cats". Archives Italiennes de Biologie 106 (3): 283–93. September 1968. PMID 5724423. 
  97. "A two process model of sleep regulation". Human Neurobiology 1 (3): 195–204. 1982. PMID 7185792. 
  98. "Effect of SCN lesions on sleep in squirrel monkeys: evidence for opponent processes in sleep-wake regulation". The Journal of Neuroscience 13 (3): 1065–79. March 1993. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01065.1993. PMID 8441003. 
  99. Birendra N. Mallick, ed (2011-07-14). Rapid eye movement sleep : regulation and function. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521116800. 
  100. "Sleep and arousal: thalamocortical mechanisms". Annual Review of Neuroscience 20 (1): 185–215. 1 March 1997. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.185. PMID 9056712. 
  101. "Sleep-wake cycle abnormalities in fatal familial insomnia. Evidence of the role of the thalamus in sleep regulation". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 94 (6): 398–405. June 1995. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(94)00318-F. PMID 7607093. 
  102. "The thalamus participates in the regulation of the sleep-waking cycle. A clinico-pathological study in fatal familial thalamic degeneration". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 73 (2): 117–23. August 1989. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(89)90190-9. PMID 2473878. 
  103. "The sleep relay--the role of the thalamus in central and decentral sleep regulation". Pflügers Archiv 463 (1): 53–71. January 2012. doi:10.1007/s00424-011-1014-6. PMID 21912835. 
  104. "Neurophysiological foundations of sleep, arousal, awareness and consciousness phenomena. Part 1". Anaesthesiology Intensive Therapy 47 (2): 162–7. 2015. doi:10.5603/AIT.2015.0015. PMID 25940332. "The ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) is responsible for a sustained wakefulness state. It receives information from sensory receptors of various modalities, transmitted through spinoreticular pathways and cranial nerves (trigeminal nerve — polymodal pathways, olfactory nerve, optic nerve and vestibulocochlear nerve — monomodal pathways). These pathways reach the thalamus directly or indirectly via the medial column of reticular formation nuclei (magnocellular nuclei and reticular nuclei of pontine tegmentum). The reticular activating system begins in the dorsal part of the posterior midbrain and anterior pons, continues into the diencephalon, and then divides into two parts reaching the thalamus and hypothalamus, which then project into the cerebral cortex (Fig. 1). The thalamic projection is dominated by cholinergic neurons originating from the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of pons and midbrain (PPT) and laterodorsal tegmental nucleus of pons and midbrain (LDT) nuclei [17, 18]. The hypothalamic projection involves noradrenergic neurons of the locus coeruleus (LC) and serotoninergic neurons of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei (DR), which pass through the lateral hypothalamus and reach axons of the histaminergic tubero-mamillary nucleus (TMN), together forming a pathway extending into the forebrain, cortex and hippocampus. Cortical arousal also takes advantage of dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra (SN), ventral tegmenti area (VTA) and the periaqueductal grey area (PAG). Fewer cholinergic neurons of the pons and midbrain send projections to the forebrain along the ventral pathway, bypassing the thalamus [19, 20].". 
  105. "Chapter 12: Sleep and Arousal". Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York, USA: McGraw-Hill Medical. 2009. p. 295. ISBN 9780071481274. "The RAS is a complex structure consisting of several different circuits including the four monoaminergic pathways ... The norepinephrine pathway originates from the locus ceruleus (LC) and related brainstem nuclei; the serotonergic neurons originate from the raphe nuclei within the brainstem as well; the dopaminergic neurons originate in ventral tegmental area (VTA); and the histaminergic pathway originates from neurons in the tuberomammillary nucleus (TMN) of the posterior hypothalamus. As discussed in Chapter 6, these neurons project widely throughout the brain from restricted collections of cell bodies. Norepinephrine, serotonin, dopamine, and histamine have complex modulatory functions and, in general, promote wakefulness. The PT in the brain stem is also an important component of the ARAS. Activity of PT cholinergic neurons (REM-on cells) promotes REM sleep. During waking, REM-on cells are inhibited by a subset of ARAS norepinephrine and serotonin neurons called REM-off cells." 
  106. "The ascending mesolimbic cholinergic system--a specific division of the reticular activating system involved in the initiation of negative emotional states". Journal of Molecular Neuroscience 53 (3): 436–45. July 2014. doi:10.1007/s12031-013-0179-1. PMID 24272957. "Understanding of arousing and wakefulness-maintaining functions of the ARAS has been further complicated by neurochemical discoveries of numerous groups of neurons with the ascending pathways originating within the brainstem reticular core, including pontomesencephalic nuclei, which synthesize different transmitters and release them in vast areas of the brain and in the entire neocortex (for review, see Jones 2003; Lin et al. 2011). They included glutamatergic, cholinergic, noradrenergic, dopaminergic, serotonergic, histaminergic, and orexinergic systems (for review, see Lin et al. 2011). ... The ARAS represented diffuse, nonspecific pathways that, working through the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei, could change activity of the entire neocortex, and thus, this system was suggested initially as a general arousal system to natural stimuli and the critical system underlying wakefulness (Moruzzi and Magoun 1949; Lindsley et al. 1949; Starzl et al. 1951, see stippled area in Fig. 1). ... It was found in a recent study in the rat that the state of wakefulness is mostly maintained by the ascending glutamatergic projection from the parabrachial nucleus and precoeruleus regions to the basal forebrain and then relayed to the cerebral cortex (Fuller et al. 2011). ... Anatomical studies have shown two main pathways involved in arousal and originating from the areas with cholinergic cell groups, one through the thalamus and the other, traveling ventrally through the hypothalamus and preoptic area, and reciprocally connected with the limbic system (Nauta and Kuypers 1958; Siegel 2004). ... As counted in the cholinergic connections to the thalamic reticular nucleus ...". 
  107. "The Neurobiology of Sleep and Wakefulness". The Psychiatric Clinics of North America 38 (4): 615–44. December 2015. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2015.07.002. PMID 26600100. "This ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) is comprised of cholinergic laterodorsal and pedunculopontine tegmentum (LDT/PPT), noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC), serotonergic (5-HT) Raphe nuclei and dopaminergic ventral tegmental area (VTA), substantia nigra (SN) and periaqueductal gray projections that stimulate the cortex directly and indirectly via the thalamus, hypothalamus and BF.6, 12-18 These aminergic and catecholaminergic populations have numerous interconnections and parallel projections which likely impart functional redundancy and resilience to the system.6, 13, 19 ... More recently, the medullary parafacial zone (PZ) adjacent to the facial nerve was identified as a sleep-promoting center on the basis of anatomical, electrophysiological and chemo- and optogenetic studies.23, 24 GABAergic PZ neurons inhibit glutamatergic parabrachial (PB) neurons that project to the BF,25 thereby promoting NREM sleep at the expense of wakefulness and REM sleep. ... The Hcrt neurons project widely throughout the brain and spinal cord92, 96, 99, 100 including major projections to wake-promoting cell groups such as the HA cells of the TM,101 the 5-HT cells of the dorsal Raphe nuclei (DRN),101 the noradrenergic cells of the LC,102 and cholinergic cells in the LDT, PPT, and BF.101, 103 ... Hcrt directly excites cellular systems involved in waking and arousal including the LC,102, 106, 107 DRN,108, 109 TM,110-112 LDT,113, 114 cholinergic BF,115 and both dopamine (DA) and non-DA neurons in the VTA.116, 117". 
  108. "Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 9 (12): 910–9. December 2008. doi:10.1038/nrn2521. PMID 18985047. 
  109. Ciba Foundation symposium on the nature of sleep. Boston: Little, Brown. 1961. ISBN 978-0-470-71922-0. 
  110. "Sleep Syllabus. B. The Phylogeny of Sleep". Sleep Research Society, Education Committee. http://www.sleephomepages.org/sleepsyllabus/b.html. 
  111. "Function of Sleep.". Scribd.com. Retrieved on 1 December 2011.
  112. "Why we sleep: a theoretical view of sleep function". Sleep Medicine Reviews 3 (2): 119–29. June 1999. doi:10.1016/S1087-0792(99)90019-9. PMID 15310481. 
  113. "A neuronal group theory of sleep function". Journal of Sleep Research 2 (2): 63–69. June 1993. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.1993.tb00064.x. PMID 10607073. 
  114. "Short- and long-term health consequences of sleep disruption" (in en). Nature and Science of Sleep 9: 151–161. 2017-05-19. doi:10.2147/nss.s134864. PMID 28579842. 
  115. Choi, Charles Q. (25 August 2009) New Theory Questions Why We Sleep, LiveScience.com.
  116. "Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain". Science 342 (6156): 373–7. October 2013. doi:10.1126/science.1241224. PMID 24136970. Bibcode2013Sci...342..373X. 
  117. 117.0 117.1 117.2 "BRAIN DRAIN". Scientific American 314 (3): 44–9. March 2016. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0316-44. PMID 27066643. Bibcode2016SciAm.314c..44N. 
  118. 118.0 118.1 "Physiology of blood-brain interfaces in relation to brain disposition of small compounds and macromolecules". Molecular Pharmaceutics 10 (5): 1473–91. May 2013. doi:10.1021/mp300518e. PMID 23298398. 
  119. "Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats". The International Journal of Neuroscience 114 (11): 1433–42. November 2004. doi:10.1080/00207450490509168. PMID 15636354. http://osf.io/jshra/. 
  120. "Effects of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats". American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 293 (1): R504–9. July 2007. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00105.2007. PMID 17409265. 
  121. "Sleeping to fuel the immune system: mammalian sleep and resistance to parasites". BMC Evolutionary Biology 9: 8. January 2009. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-8. PMID 19134176. 
  122. Peres, Judy (14 March 2012) A good reason to get your zzz's Chicago Tribune Health, retrieved 26 March 2014
  123. "Sleep duration from ages 1 to 10 years: variability and stability in comparison with growth". Pediatrics 120 (4): e769–76. October 2007. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3300. PMID 17908734. 
  124. "Brain may flush out toxins during sleep". National Institutes of Health. http://www.ninds.nih.gov/news_and_events/news_articles/pressrelease_brain_sleep_10182013.htm. 
  125. "Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep". Nature 437 (7063): 1264–71. October 2005. doi:10.1038/nature04285. PMID 16251951. Bibcode2005Natur.437.1264S. 
  126. 126.0 126.1 "Perchance to prune. During sleep, the brain weakens the connections among nerve cells, apparently conserving energy and, paradoxically, aiding memory". Scientific American 309 (2): 34–9. August 2013. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0813-34. PMID 23923204. Bibcode2013SciAm.309b..34T. 
  127. "Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans". Science 210 (4475): 1267–9. December 1980. doi:10.1126/science.7434030. PMID 7434030. Bibcode1980Sci...210.1267L. 
  128. "Transition from dim to bright light in the morning induces an immediate elevation of cortisol levels". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 86 (1): 151–7. January 2001. doi:10.1210/jcem.86.1.7102. PMID 11231993. 
  129. "A quantitative estimation of growth hormone secretion in normal man: reproducibility and relation to sleep and time of day". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 74 (6): 1441–50. June 1992. doi:10.1210/jcem.74.6.1592892. PMID 1592892. 
  130. "Changes in cortisol and growth hormone secretion during nocturnal sleep in the course of aging". The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 51 (1): M3–9. January 1996. doi:10.1093/gerona/51A.1.M3. PMID 8548511. 
  131. 131.0 131.1 "The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation". Sleep Medicine Reviews 11 (3): 163–78. June 2007. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002. PMID 17442599. 
  132. "Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function". Lancet 354 (9188): 1435–9. October 1999. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(99)01376-8. PMID 10543671. 
  133. "Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9 (4): 534–547. 1997. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.534. PMID 23968216. 
  134. 134.0 134.1 134.2 134.3 "Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation". Science 315 (5817): 1426–9. 2007. doi:10.1126/science.1138581. PMID 17347444. Bibcode2007Sci...315.1426R. 
  135. 135.0 135.1 135.2 135.3 135.4 "System consolidation of memory during sleep". Psychological Research 76 (2): 192–203. 2012. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6. PMID 21541757. 
  136. "The memory function of sleep". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11 (2): 114–126. 2010. doi:10.1038/nrn2762. PMID 20046194. 
  137. 137.0 137.1 "About sleep's role in memory". Physiological Reviews 93 (2): 681–766. 2013. doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012. PMID 23589831. 
  138. 138.0 138.1 "Boosting Vocabulary Learning by Verbal Cueing During Sleep". Cerebral Cortex 25 (11): 4169–4179. 2015. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu139. PMID 24962994. 
  139. 139.0 139.1 139.2 "The beneficial role of memory reactivation for language learning during sleep: A review". Brain and Language 167: 94–105. 2017. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2016.02.005. PMID 27036946. http://doc.rero.ch/record/323249/files/schreinerrasch2017_brainlang.pdf. 
  140. 140.0 140.1 "Auditory Closed-Loop Stimulation of the Sleep Slow Oscillation Enhances Memory". Neuron 78 (3): 545–553. 2013. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.006. PMID 23583623. 
  141. "Odor cueing during slow-wave sleep benefits memory independently of low cholinergic tone". Psychopharmacology 235 (1): 291–299. 2018. doi:10.1007/s00213-017-4768-5. PMID 29119218. 
  142. "Theta Phase-Coordinated Memory Reactivation Reoccurs in a Slow-Oscillatory Rhythm during NREM Sleep". Cell Reports 25 (2): 296–301. 2018. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.037. PMID 30304670. 
  143. "Cueing vocabulary during sleep increases theta activity during later recognition testing". Psychophysiology 52 (11): 1538–1543. 2015. doi:10.1111/psyp.12505. PMID 26235609. 
  144. Groch, S., Schreiner, T., Rasch, B., Huber, R., & Wilhelm, I. (2017). Prior knowledge is essential for the beneficial effect of targeted memory reactivation during sleep. Scientific Reports, 7
  145. Tononi G (2011). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis (Conference talk). Allen Institute.
  146. Psychology The Science of Behavior, Books a La Carte Edition.. Pearson College Div. 2010. ISBN 978-0-205-76223-1. 
  147. "To sleep, no doubt to dream…". Forty Studies That Changed Psychology (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. 2013. pp. 42–49. ISBN 978-0-205-91839-3. 
  148. "Chapter 12: The Effect of Dream Deprivation: The need for a certain amount of dreaming each night is suggested by recent experiments". Brian Physiology and Psychology. University of California Press. 1966. 
  149. 149.0 149.1 "The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation". Sleep 26 (2): 117–26. March 2003. doi:10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. PMID 12683469. 
  150. "Driver sleepiness and risk of serious injury to car occupants: population based case control study". BMJ 324 (7346): 1125. May 2002. doi:10.1136/bmj.324.7346.1125. PMID 12003884. 
  151. "Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1129 (1): 305–22. 1 May 2008. doi:10.1196/annals.1417.002. PMID 18591490. Bibcode2008NYASA1129..305L. 
  152. "Combination of bright light and caffeine as a countermeasure for impaired alertness and performance during extended sleep deprivation". Journal of Sleep Research 6 (1): 26–35. March 1997. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00022.x. PMID 9125696. 
  153. 153.0 153.1 "Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation". Seminars in Neurology 25 (1): 117–29. March 2005. doi:10.1055/s-2005-867080. PMID 15798944. 
  154. "Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation". Journal of Sleep Research 10 (2): 85–92. June 2001. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00245.x. PMID 11422722. 
  155. "Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions". Sleep 33 (3): 335–42. March 2010. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.3.335. PMID 20337191. 
  156. "Sleep and depression". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66 (10): 1254–69. October 2005. doi:10.4088/JCP.v66n1008. PMID 16259539. 
  157. "Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition". Current Opinion in Neurology 21 (4): 417–23. August 2008. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e3283052cf7. PMID 18607201. 
  158. 158.0 158.1 "Best practice guide for the treatment of REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD)". Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 6 (1): 85–95. 2010. doi:10.5664/jcsm.27717. PMID 20191945. 
  159. 159.0 159.1 "REM sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson's disease – Is there a gender difference?". Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 19 (1): 120–122. 2013. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.05.027. PMID 22726815. 
  160. 160.0 160.1 "Longitudinal assessment of probable rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder in Parkinson's disease". European Journal of Neurology 22 (8): 1242–1244. 2015. doi:10.1111/ene.12723. PMID 25904103. 
  161. 161.0 161.1 161.2 "Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease". Aging Clin Exp Res 28 (5): 951–957. 2016. doi:10.1007/s40520-015-0382-8. PMID 26022447. 
  162. 162.0 162.1 162.2 162.3 "REM Sleep Behavior Disorder and Other Sleep Disturbances in Non-Alzheimer Dementias". Current Sleep Medicine Reports 3 (3): 193–203. 2017. doi:10.1007/s40675-017-0078-z. 
  163. 163.0 163.1 163.2 163.3 163.4 163.5 163.6 Dick-Muehlke, C. (2015). Psychosocial studies of the individual's changing perspectives in Alzheimer's disease (Premier Reference Source). Hershey, PA: Medical Information Science Reference.
  164. "The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (6): 877–901; discussion 904–1121. December 2000. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00004015. PMID 11515147. 
  165. "A more general evolutionary hypothesis about dream function". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (6): 972–973. 2000. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00664026. 
  166. "Sleep, learning, and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing". Science 294 (5544): 1052–7. November 2001. doi:10.1126/science.1063530. PMID 11691983. Bibcode2001Sci...294.1052S. 
  167. "A research-based reconsideration of the psychoanalytic theory of dreaming". Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 40 (2): 531–50. 1992. doi:10.1177/000306519204000211. PMID 1593084. 
  168. "Effects of stress on sleep: the moderating role of coping style". Health Psychology 23 (5): 542–5. September 2004. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.23.5.542. PMID 15367075. 
  169. "Nightmares: a new neurocognitive model". Sleep Medicine Reviews 11 (4): 295–310. August 2007. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.004. PMID 17498981. 
  170. "Drugs, Dreams, and Nightmares". Sleep Medicine Clinics 5 (2): 277–287. 2010. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2010.01.007. 
  171. "Nightmares, Bad Dreams, and Emotion Dysregulation: A Review and New Neurocognitive Model of Dreaming". Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 (2): 84–88. 1 April 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01614.x. 




Licensed under CC BY-SA 3.0 | Source: https://handwiki.org/wiki/Biology:Neuroscience_of_sleep
12 views | Status: cached on October 06 2024 08:37:23
↧ Download this article as ZWI file
Encyclosphere.org EncycloReader is supported by the EncyclosphereKSF