Sex differences in cognition

From HandWiki - Reading time: 17 min

Sex differences in cognition are widely studied in the current scientific literature. Biological and genetic differences in combination with environment and culture have resulted in the cognitive differences among males and females. Among biological factors, hormones such as testosterone and estrogen may play some role mediating these differences. Among differences of diverse mental and cognitive abilities, the largest or most well known are those relating to spatial abilities, social cognition and verbal skills and abilities.

Cognitive abilities

Cognitive abilities are mental abilities that a person uses in everyday life, as well as specific demand tasks. The most basic of these abilities are memory, executive function, processing speed and perception, which combine to form a larger perceptual umbrella relating to different social, affective, verbal and spatial information. Memory, which is one of the primary core of cognitive abilities can be broken down into short-term memory, working memory and long-term memory. There are also other abilities relating to perceptual information such as mental rotation, spatial visualization ability, verbal fluency and reading comprehension. Other larger perceptual umbrellas include social cognition, empathy, spatial perception and verbal abilities.

Sex differences in memory

Main page: Philosophy:Sex differences in memory

Short term memory

Various researchers have conducted studies to determine the differences between males and females and their abilities within their short-term memory. For example, a study conducted by Lowe, Mayfield, and Reynolds (2003) examined sex differences among children and adolescents on various short-term memory measures. This study included 1,279 children and adolescents, 637 males and 642 females, between the ages of 5 and 19. They found that females scored higher on two verbal subtests: Word Selective Reminding and Object Recall, and males scored higher on the Memory for Location and Abstract Visual Memory subtests, the key spatial memory tasks. In two different studies researchers have found that women perform higher on verbal tasks and men perform higher on spatial tasks (Voyer, Voyer, & Saint-Aubin, 2016). These findings are consistent with studies of intelligence with regards to pattern, females performing higher on certain verbal tasks and males performing higher on certain spatial tasks (Voyer, Voyer, & Saint-Aubin, 2016). Same results have been also found cross culturally.[1] Sex differences in verbal short-term memory have been found regardless of age even among adults, for example a review published in the journal Neuropsychologia which evaluated studies from 1990 to 2013 found greater female verbal memory from ages 11–89 years old.[2][3]

Working memory

There are usually no sex differences in overall working memory except those involving spatial information such as space and object. A 2004 study published in the journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology found significantly higher male performance on four visuo-spatial working memory.[4] Another 2010 study published in the journal Brain and Cognition found a male advantage in spatial and object working memory on an n-back test but not for verbal working memory.[5] Similarly another study published in the journal Human Brain Mapping found no sex differences in a verbal n-back working memory task among adults from ages 18–58 years old.[6] There was also no sex differences in verbal working memory among a study of university students published in the Journal of Dental and Medical Sciences. However, they still found greater male spatial working memory in studies published in the journals Brain Cognition and Intelligence.[7][8] Also, even though they found no sex differences in verbal working memory, researchers have found lower brain activity or thermodynamics in the prefrontal cortex of females which suggested greater neural efficiency and less effort for the same performance.[9] Researchers indicate females might have greater working memory on tasks that only relies on the prefrontal cortex.[9] However, in another study of working memory, where the goal was to detect sex differences under high loads of working memory, males outperformed females under high loads of working memory. The authors of the study state: "Results indicated sex effects at high loads across tasks and within each task, such that males had higher accuracy, even among groups that were matched for performance at lower loads".[10] A 2006 review and study on working memory published in the journal European Journal of Cognitive Psychology also found no sex differences in working memory processes except in a double-span task where females outperformed males.[11] There have also been no sex differences found in a popular working memory task known as n-back among a large number of studies.[9][11][12][13][14]

Long term memory

Studies have found a greater female ability in episodic memory involving verbal or both verbal and visual-spatial tasks while a higher male ability that only involves complex visual-spatial episodic memory.[15][16] For example, a study published in the journal Neuropsychology found that women perform at a higher level on most verbal episodic tasks and tasks involving some or little visual-spatial episodic memory.[17] Another study published the following year found that women perform at a higher level in verbal and non-verbal (non-spatial visual) episodic memory but men performed at a higher level in complex visual-spatial episodic memory.[18] A review published in the journal Current Directions in Psychological Science by researcher Agneta Herlitz also conclude that higher ability in women on episodic-memory tasks requiring both verbal and visuospatial episodic memory and on face-recognition tasks, while men have higher abilities for episodic memory, where visual-spatial skills of high complexity are required.[15]

Sex differences in semantic memory have also been found with a higher female ability which can be explained by a female advantage in verbal fluency.[19] One other study also found greater female free-recall and long term retrieval among the ages 5–17.[20]

In another study, when using multiple tests for episodic memory, there were no differences between men and women.[21] A similar result was also found among children from 3 to 6 years old.[22] As for semantic memory related to general knowledge and knowledge of facts from the world. That is, in most areas of cognition, men show higher results on semantic memory.[23][24][25][26][27]

Sex differences in executive functions

There has not been enough literature or studies assessing sex difference in executive functioning, especially since executive functions are not a unitary concept. However, in the ones that have been done, there have been differences found in attention and inhibition.

Attention

A 2002 study published in the Journal of Vision found that males were faster at shifting attention from one object to another as well as shifting attention within objects.[28] 2012–2014 studies published in the Journal of Neuropsychology with a sample size ranging from 3500 to 9138 participants by researcher Ruben C Gur found higher female attention accuracy in a neurocognitive battery assessing individuals from ages 8–21.[29][30] A 2013 study published in the Chinese Medical Journal found no sex differences in executive and alerting of attention networks but faster orientation of attention among females.[31] A 2010 study published in Neuropsychologia also found greater female responsiveness in attention to processing overall sensory stimulation.[32]

Inhibition and self-regulation

A 2008 study published in the journal Psychophysiology found faster reaction time to deviant stimuli in women.[33] The study also analyzed past literature and found higher female performance in withholding social behavior such as aggressive responses and improper sexual arousal.[33] Furthermore, they found evidence that women were better at resisting temptation in tasks, delaying gratification and controlling emotional expressions.[33] They also found lower female effort in response inhibition in equal performance for the same tasks implying an advantage for females in response inhibition based on neural efficiency.[33] In another study published in 2011 in the journal Brain and Cognition, it was found that females outperformed males on the Sustained Attention to Response Task which is a test that measures inhibitory control.[34] Researchers have hypothesized that any female advantage in inhibition or self-regulation may have evolved as a response to greater parenting responsibilities in ancestral settings.[33]

Sex differences in processing speed

Sex differences in processing speed has been largely noted in literature. Studies published in the journal Intelligence have found faster processing speed in women. For example, a 2006 study published in Intelligence by researcher Stephen Camarata and Richard Woodcock found faster processing speed in females across all age groups in a sample of 4,213 participants.[35] This was followed by another study published in 2008 by researchers Timothy Z Keith and Matthew R. Reynolds who found faster processing speed in females from ages 6 to 89 years old.[36] The sample also had a number of 8,818 participants.[36] Other studies by Keith have also found faster processing speed in females from ages 5 to 17.[20] In one recent study, groups of men and women were tested using the WAIS-IV and WAIS-R tests. According to the research results, there were no differences in processing speed between men and women.[37]

Sex differences in semantic perception

Studies of sex differences in semantic perception (attribution of meaning) of words reported that males conceptualize items in terms of physical or observable attributes whereas females use more evaluative concepts.[38][39] Another study of young adults in three cultures showed significant sex differences in semantic perception (attribution of meaning) of most common and abstract words. Contrary to common beliefs, women gave more negative scores to the concepts describing sensational objects, social and physical attractors but more positive estimations to work- and reality-related words, in comparison to men [40] This suggests that men favour concepts related to extreme experience and women favour concepts related to predictable and controllable routines. In a light of the higher rates of sensation seeking and deviancy in males, in comparison to females, these sex differences in meaning attribution were interpreted as support for the evolutionary theory of sex.[41]

Sex differences in spatial abilities

Rubik's cube puzzle involving mental rotation

Sex differences in spatial abilities are widely established in literature. Males have much higher level of performance in three major spatial tasks which include spatial visualization, spatial perception and mental rotation.[42][43] Spatial visualization elicits the smallest difference with a deviation of 0.13, perception a deviation of 0.44 and mental rotation the largest with a deviation of 0.73.[42][44][45] Another 2013 meta-analysis published in the journal Educational Review found greater male mental rotation in a deviation of 0.57 which only grew larger as time limits were added.[46] These male advantages manifests themselves in math and mechanical tasks for example significantly higher male performance on tests of geometry, measurement, probability, statistics and especially mechanical reasoning.[47] It also manifests and largely mediates higher male performance in arithmetic and computational fluency[48] All of these math and technical fields involve spatial abilities such as rotation and manipulation of imagined space, symbols and objects. Mental rotation has also been linked to higher success in fields of engineering, physics and chemistry regardless of gender.[49] Spatial visualization on the other hand also correlate with higher math achievement in a range of 0.30 to 0.60.[50] Furthermore, male advantage in spatial abilities can be accounted for by their greater ability in spatial working memory.[7] Sex differences in mental rotation also reaches almost a single deviation (1.0) when the tasks require navigation, as found in one study with participants who used Oculus Rift in a virtual environment.[51]

Even though most spatial abilities are higher in men, object location memory or the ability to memorize spatial cues involving categorical relations are higher in women.[2][52] But it depends on the type of stimulus (object) and the task. In some conditions, men's productivity is higher (for example, when "male" objects are used), in other conditions, women's productivity may be higher or there are no differences between the sexes.[52] Higher female ability in visual recognition of objects and shapes have also been found.[53][54]


Sex differences in verbal abilities

Like spatial ability, sex differences in verbal abilities have been widely established in literature. There is a clear higher female performance on a number of verbal tasks prominently a higher level of performance in speech production which reaches a deviation of 0.33 and also a higher performance in writing.[43][44][55][56][57] Studies have also found greater female performance in phonological processing, identifying alphabetical sequences, and word fluency tasks.[58][59][60] Studies have also found that females outperform males in verbal learning especially on tests such as Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and Verbal Paired Associates.[61][62] It has also been found that the hormone estrogen increases ability of speech production and phonological processing in women, which could be tied to their advantages in these areas.[47] Overall better female performance have also been found in verbal fluency which include a trivial advantage reading comprehension while a significantly higher performance in speech production and essay writing.[49][55] This manifests in higher female international PISA scores in reading and higher female Grade 12 scores in national reading, writing and study skills.[63][64] Researchers Joseph M. Andreano and Larry Cahill have also found that the female verbal advantage extends into numerous tasks, including tests of spatial and autobiographical abilities.[16]

In a fairly large meta-analysis that analyzed 165 different studies, a very small difference of 0.11 standard deviations was found. The authors of this study postulate: "The difference is so small that we argue that gender differences in verbal ability no longer exist."[65]

Sex differences in social cognition

Current literature suggests women have higher level of social cognition. A 2012 review published in the journal Neuropsychologia found that women are better at recognizing facial effects, expression processing and emotions in general.[66] Men were only better at recognizing specific behaviour which includes anger, aggression and threatening cues.[66] A 2012 study published in the journal Neuropsychology with a sample of 3,500 individuals from ages 8–21, found that females outperformed males on face memory and all social cognition tests.[29] In 2014, another study published in the journal Cerebral Cortex found that females had larger activity in the right temporal cortex, an essential core of the social brain connected to perception and understanding the social behaviour of others such as intentions, emotions, and expectations.[67] In 2014, a meta-analysis of 215 study sample by researcher A.E. Johnson and D Voyeur in the journal Cognition and Emotion found overall female advantage in emotional recognition.[68] Other studies have also indicated greater female superiority to discriminate vocal and facial expression regardless of valence, and also being able to accurately process emotional speech.[69] Studies have also found males to be slower in making social judgments than females.[70] Structural studies with MRI neuroimaging has also shown that women have bigger regional grey matter volumes in a number of regions related to social information processing including the Inferior frontal cortex and bigger cortical folding in the Inferior frontal cortex and parietal cortex [70] Researchers suppose that these sex differences in social cognition predisposes males to high rates of autism spectrum disorders which is characterized by lower social cognition.[70]

A recent study that aimed to identify gender differences in social cognition did not show significant differences, with few exceptions. The study authors state: "The presence of sex differences in social cognition is controversial".[71] Results showed no significant sex differences in affective and cognitive ToM, in the recognition of emotional facial expressions, or in the ability to identify and regulate one's own emotions.[72]

Empathy

Empathy is a large part of social cognition and facilitates its cognitive components known as theory of mind. Current literature suggests a higher level of empathy in women compared to men.[73][74][75]

A 2014 analysis from the journal of Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews reported that there is evidence that "sex differences in empathy have phylogenetic and ontogenetic roots in biology and are not merely cultural byproducts driven by socialization."[74] Other research has found no differences in empathy between women and men, and suggest that perceived gender differences are the result of motivational differences.[76][77][78]

See also

References

  1. "Sex differences in cognitive abilities: a cross-cultural perspective". Neuropsychologia 28 (10): 1063–77. 1990-01-01. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(90)90141-A. PMID 2267058. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 "Why women see differently from the way men see? A review of sex differences in cognition and sports". Journal of Sport and Health Science 3 (3): 155–162. September 2014. doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2014.03.012. PMID 25520851. 
  3. Kramer, Joel H.; Delis, Dean C.; Daniel, Mark (1988). "Sex differences in verbal learning". Journal of Clinical Psychology 44 (6): 907–915. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198811)44:6<907::AID-JCLP2270440610>3.0.CO;2-8. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246873849. 
  4. "Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies". Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 (5): 519–532. July 2004. doi:10.1002/acp.1000. PMID 20676381. 
  5. "A male advantage for spatial and object but not verbal working memory using the n-back task". Brain and Cognition 76 (1): 191–6. June 2011. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.12.002. PMID 21411205. 
  6. "No gender differences in brain activation during the N-back task: an fMRI study in healthy individuals". Human Brain Mapping 30 (11): 3609–15. November 2009. doi:10.1002/hbm.20783. PMID 19387979. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 "Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity?". Intelligence 35 (3): 211–223. 2007. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.009. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222398504. 
  8. "A sex difference on a novel spatial working memory task in humans". Brain and Cognition 47 (3): 470–93. December 2001. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1326. PMID 11748902. 
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 "Gender-specific hemodynamics in prefrontal cortex during a verbal working memory task by near-infrared spectroscopy". Behavioural Brain Research 209 (1): 148–53. May 2010. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.033. PMID 20117145. 
  10. Reed, Jessica L.; Gallagher, Natalie M.; Sullivan, Marie; Callicott, Joseph H.; Green, Adam E. (2017-04-01). "Sex differences in verbal working memory performance emerge at very high loads of common neuroimaging tasks" (in en). Brain and Cognition 113: 56–64. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2017.01.001. ISSN 0278-2626. PMID 28119206. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 "Are there gender differences in verbal and visuospatial working-memory resources?". European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 18 (3): 378–397. 2006. doi:10.1080/09541440500234104. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247497425. 
  12. "N -back task to assess sex difference in working memory: A pilot study". https://www.researchgate.net/publication/287278868. 
  13. "Sex differences in working memory". Psychological Reports 103 (1): 214–8. August 2008. doi:10.2466/PR0.103.5.214-218. PMID 18982954. 
  14. "Sex differences in prefrontal cortical brain activity during fMRI of auditory verbal working memory". Neuropsychology 19 (4): 509–19. July 2005. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.509. PMID 16060826. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 "Sex Differences in Episodic Memory". Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 (1): 52–56. 2008. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00547.x. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228618349. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 "Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory". Learning & Memory 16 (4): 248–66. April 2009. doi:10.1101/lm.918309. PMID 19318467. 
  17. "Sex differences in episodic memory: the impact of verbal and visuospatial ability". Neuropsychology 13 (4): 590–7. October 1999. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.590. PMID 10527068. 
  18. "Sex differences favoring women in verbal but not in visuospatial episodic memory". Neuropsychology 15 (2): 165–73. April 2001. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.165. PMID 11324860. 
  19. "Selective sex differences in declarative memory". Memory & Cognition 32 (7): 1160–9. October 2004. doi:10.3758/BF03196889. PMID 15813497. 
  20. 20.0 20.1 "Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages 5 to 17: Evidence from the Differential Ability Scales—Second Edition". Intelligence 39 (5): 389–404. 2011. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.06.008. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232383641. 
  21. Cheke, Lucy G.; Clayton, Nicola S. (2013-09-01). "Do different tests of episodic memory produce consistent results in human adults?" (in en). Learning & Memory 20 (9): 491–498. doi:10.1101/lm.030502.113. ISSN 1072-0502. PMID 23955172. http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/20/9/491. 
  22. Cheke, Lucy G.; Clayton, Nicola S. (September 2015). "The six blind men and the elephant: Are episodic memory tasks tests of different things or different tests of the same thing?". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 137: 164–171. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.006. ISSN 0022-0965. PMID 25931424. 
  23. Lynn, Richard; Irwing, Paul (November 2002). "Sex differences in general knowledge, semantic memory and reasoning ability". British Journal of Psychology 93 (Pt 4): 545–556. doi:10.1348/000712602761381394. ISSN 0007-1269. PMID 12519533. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12519533/. 
  24. Lynn, Richard; Ivanec, Dragutin; Zarevski, Predrag (June 2009). "Sex differences in general knowledge domains". Collegium Antropologicum 33 (2): 515–520. ISSN 0350-6134. PMID 19662772. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19662772/. 
  25. Lynn, Richard; Irwing, Paul; Cammock, Thomas (2001-01-01). "Sex differences in general knowledge" (in en). Intelligence 30 (1): 27–39. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00064-2. ISSN 0160-2896. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0160289601000642. 
  26. Lynn, Richard; Wilberg-Neidhardt, Sylwia; Margraf-Stiksrud, Jutta (2005-12-01). "Sex differences in general knowledge in German and Northern Irish university students". Sexualities, Evolution & Gender 7 (3): 277–285. doi:10.1080/14616660500477755. ISSN 1479-2508. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616660500477755. 
  27. Tran, Ulrich S.; Hofer, Agnes A.; Voracek, Martin (2014). "Sex differences in general knowledge: meta-analysis and new data on the contribution of school-related moderators among high-school students". PLOS ONE 9 (10): e110391. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110391. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 25347190. Bibcode2014PLoSO...9k0391T. 
  28. "JOV | Sex differences in shifting attention within and between objects". Journal of Vision 2 (7): 13. 2010. doi:10.1167/2.7.13. 
  29. 29.0 29.1 "Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21". Neuropsychology 26 (2): 251–265. March 2012. doi:10.1037/a0026712. PMID 22251308. 
  30. "Within-individual variability in neurocognitive performance: age- and sex-related differences in children and youths from ages 8 to 21". Neuropsychology 28 (4): 506–18. July 2014. doi:10.1037/neu0000067. PMID 24773417. 
  31. "Gender differences associated with orienting attentional networks in healthy subjects". Chinese Medical Journal 126 (12): 2308–12. June 2013. PMID 23786944. 
  32. "Sex differences in sensorimotor mu rhythms during selective attentional processing". Neuropsychologia 48 (14): 4102–10. December 2010. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.016. PMID 20951711. 
  33. 33.0 33.1 33.2 33.3 33.4 "Gender differences in behavioral inhibitory control: ERP evidence from a two-choice oddball task". Psychophysiology 45 (6): 986–93. November 2008. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00693.x. PMID 18778319. 
  34. "Inhibitory control and empathy-related personality traits: sex-linked associations". Brain and Cognition 76 (3): 364–8. August 2011. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.04.004. PMID 21570758. 
  35. "Sex differences in processing speed: Developmental effects in males and females". Intelligence 34 (3): 231–252. 2006. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.12.001. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222702039. 
  36. 36.0 36.1 "Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages 6 to 59: Evidence from the Woodcock–Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities". Intelligence 36 (6): 502–525. 2008. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.001. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222660770. 
  37. "Gender differences and measurement bias in the assessment of adult intelligence: Evidence from the Italian WAIS-IV and WAIS-R standardizations" (in en). Intelligence 79: 101436. 2020-03-01. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101436. ISSN 0160-2896. 
  38. "Male and female spoken language difficulties: stereotypes and evidence.". Psychological Bulletin 86 (3): 616–626. 1979. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.616. 
  39. "Social class-sex contrasts in patterns of cognitive style: a cross-cultural replication.". Psychological Reports 50: 19–26. 1982. doi:10.2466/pr0.1982.50.1.19. 
  40. "Understanding misunderstanding: a study of sex differences in meaning attribution". Psychological Research 77 (6): 748–60. November 2013. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0462-8. PMID 23179581. 
  41. "Do Psychological Sex Differences Reflect Evolutionary Bisexual Partitioning?". The American Journal of Psychology 128 (4): 485–514. 2015. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.4.0485. PMID 26721176. 
  42. 42.0 42.1 "Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis". Child Development 56 (6): 1479–98. December 1985. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1985.tb00213.x. PMID 4075870. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 "Sex differences in cognitive functions". Personality and Individual Differences 35 (4): 863–875. 2003. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00288-X. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222512913. Retrieved 2016-02-01. 
  44. 44.0 44.1 "Gender differences in cognition: A minefield of research issues". The Irish Journal of Psychology 18 (4): 386–396. 1997. doi:10.1080/03033910.1997.1010558158. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/251566000. 
  45. "Impact of cognitive imaging and sex differences on the development of laparoscopic suturing skills". Canadian Journal of Surgery. Journal Canadien de Chirurgie 48 (5): 387–93. October 2005. PMID 16248138. 
  46. "A Meta-Analysis on Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability Measured by the Purdue Spatial Visualization Tests: Visualization of Rotations (PSVT:R)". Educational Psychology Review 25 (1): 69–94. 2012-12-09. doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9215-x. ISSN 1040-726X. 
  47. 47.0 47.1 Sex Affects Behavior and Perception. National Academies Press (US). 2001-01-01. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK222297/. 
  48. "Sex differences in spatial cognition, computational fluency, and arithmetical reasoning". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 77 (4): 337–53. December 2000. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2594. PMID 11063633. 
  49. 49.0 49.1 Women in Science and Mathematics. National Academies Press (US). 2006-01-01. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK23762/. 
  50. "Spatial visualization, visual imagery, and mathematical problem solving of students with varying abilities". Journal of Learning Disabilities 39 (6): 496–506. 2006-12-01. doi:10.1177/00222194060390060201. PMID 17165617. 
  51. "Assessing Mental Rotation Ability in a Virtual Environment with an Oculus Rift". Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 59 (1): 1849–1852. 2016. doi:10.1177/1541931215591399. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283340799. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 Voyer, Daniel; Postma, Albert; Brake, Brandy; Imperato-McGinley, Julianne (2007-02-01). "Gender differences in object location memory: A meta-analysis" (in en). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14 (1): 23–38. doi:10.3758/BF03194024. ISSN 1531-5320. PMID 17546728. 
  53. "Gender differences in incidental learning and visual recognition memory: Support for a sex difference in unconscious environmental awareness". Personality and Individual Differences 25 (2): 223–232. 1998. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00017-8. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/223599181. Retrieved 2016-01-07. 
  54. "Sex differences in visual recognition memory: support for a sex-related difference in attention in adults and children". Brain and Cognition 34 (3): 323–36. August 1997. doi:10.1006/brcg.1997.0872. PMID 9292185. 
  55. 55.0 55.1 "Gender Differences in Academic Achievement: Is Writing an Exception to the Gender Similarities Hypothesis?". The Journal of Genetic Psychology 176 (3–4): 211–34. 2015-08-01. doi:10.1080/00221325.2015.1036833. PMID 26135387. 
  56. "Genetic and environmental influences on writing and their relations to language and reading". Annals of Dyslexia 63 (1): 25–43. April 2013. doi:10.1007/s11881-011-0055-z. PMID 21842316. 
  57. "Gender Differences in Achievement in a Large, Nationally Representative Sample of Children and Adolescents". Psychology in the Schools 52 (4): 335–348. 2015. doi:10.1002/pits.21827. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/272240824. 
  58. "Sex differences in phonetic processing: speed of identification of alphabetical sequences". Perceptual and Motor Skills 85 (3 Pt 2): 1243–51. December 1997. doi:10.2466/pms.1997.85.3f.1243. PMID 9450277. 
  59. "Sex differences in phonological processes: speeded matching and word reading". Memory & Cognition 27 (2): 246–53. March 1999. doi:10.3758/bf03211409. PMID 10226435. 
  60. "Gender-stereotyping and cognitive sex differences in mixed- and same-sex groups". Archives of Sexual Behavior 43 (8): 1663–73. November 2014. doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0311-5. PMID 24923876. 
  61. "Sex differences on the WMS-III: findings concerning verbal paired associates and faces". The Clinical Neuropsychologist 14 (2): 231–5. May 2000. doi:10.1076/1385-4046(200005)14:2;1-Z;FT231. PMID 10916198. 
  62. "Sex differences on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised in the elderly: normative data in 172 participants". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 29 (5): 561–7. July 2007. doi:10.1080/13803390600864760. PMID 17564921. 
  63. "Sex differences in mathematics and reading achievement are inversely related: within- and across-nation assessment of 10 years of PISA data". PLOS ONE 8 (3): e57988. 2013-03-13. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057988. PMID 23516422. Bibcode2013PLoSO...857988S. 
  64. Office of Research and Development (1998). SAT® and Gender Differences. College Board. https://research.collegeboard.org/sites/default/files/publications/2012/7/researchinreview-1998-4-sat-gender-differences.pdf. 
  65. "Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis.". Psychological Bulletin 104: 53–69. 1988. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.104.1.53. 
  66. 66.0 66.1 "A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals". Neuropsychologia 50 (7): 1211–21. June 2012. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022. PMID 22245006. http://dare.uva.nl/personal/pure/en/publications/a-review-on-sex-differences-in-processing-emotional-signals(3aea49a9-7521-4aae-863d-6f351a012b88).html. 
  67. "Sex Differences in the Neuromagnetic Cortical Response to Biological Motion". Cerebral Cortex 25 (10): 3468–74. October 2015. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu175. PMID 25100856. 
  68. "Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis". Cognition & Emotion 28 (7): 1164–95. 2014-01-01. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875889. PMID 24400860. 
  69. "Gender differences in the activation of inferior frontal cortex during emotional speech perception". NeuroImage 21 (3): 1114–23. March 2004. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.048. PMID 15006679. 
  70. 70.0 70.1 70.2 "Social cognition, the male brain and the autism spectrum". PLOS ONE 7 (12): e49033. 2012-12-26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049033. PMID 23300517. Bibcode2012PLoSO...749033H. 
  71. Di Tella, Marialaura; Miti, Francesca; Ardito, Rita B.; Adenzato, Mauro (2020-08-01). "Social cognition and sex: Are men and women really different?" (in en). Personality and Individual Differences 162: 110045. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110045. ISSN 0191-8869. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0191886920302348. 
  72. "Social cognition and sex: Are men and women really different?". Personality and Individual Differences 162: 110045. 2020-08-01. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110045. 
  73. "Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model". The Journal of Applied Psychology 95 (1): 54–78. January 2010. doi:10.1037/a0017286. PMID 20085406. 
  74. 74.0 74.1 "Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (Pt 4): 604–27. October 2014. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001. PMID 25236781. 
  75. "Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch". Sex Roles 64 (1–2): 70–80. January 2011. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9842-y. PMID 21297854. 
  76. Ickes, William; Gesn, Paul L.; Graham, Tiffany (20 May 2005). "Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation?". Personal Relationships 7 (1): 94–109. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00006.x. 
  77. Ickles, William (15 January 1997). Empathic Accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press. p. 140. ISBN 1572301619. https://books.google.com/books?id=-7ob3yJdFqQC. 
  78. Klein, K. J. K.; Hodges, S. D. (2001). "Gender Differences, Motivation, and Empathic Accuracy: When it Pays to Understand". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 (6): 720–730. doi:10.1177/0146167201276007. 




Licensed under CC BY-SA 3.0 | Source: https://handwiki.org/wiki/Philosophy:Sex_differences_in_cognition
6 views | Status: cached on September 08 2024 09:46:35
↧ Download this article as ZWI file
Encyclosphere.org EncycloReader is supported by the EncyclosphereKSF