Style over substance Pseudoscience |
Popular pseudosciences |
Random examples |
We control what you think with Language |
Said and done |
Jargon, buzzwords, slogans |
Pseudolinguistics is the study of language in a way that falls short of academic rigor. It is the linguistic cousin of the pseudoscientific family. Of course, pseudolinguistics has strong ties with other fields such as pseudohistory and pseudoarcheology.
In linguistics, the comparative method is the accepted method to show that languages are related. Related languages should descend from a definite proto-language, and the daughter languages should have words that transform according to definite sound laws from this proto-language. For example, in Uralic languages, the Proto-Uralic language split into at least two subgroups called Finnic and Ugric, where each 'p' in Finnic is replaced by 'f' in Ugric, e.g. Finnish pata corresponds to Hungarian fazék.
In contrast, pseudolinguists usually fail to use the comparative method, relying on false or folk etymologies, spurious similarities, various other kinds of pseudoscience (such as racialist arguments), or flat-out wild speculation.
Nationalists of various stripes are especially proficient abusers of linguistics. Many like to make up family relationships between languages or language families and then claim the newly discovered kin as a part of their nation. For example, a Turkish linguist might claim that the Finno-Ugric languages are related to Turkish and that therefore the Finns are actually Turks (for some reason, you'll rarely hear it the other way around, that my language is related to your language and therefore my people don't exist but are members of your people[note 1]). Language isolates (i.e. languages not demonstrably related to any others, such as Basque, Ainu, or many Native American languages) seem to be particularly popular choices for such theories, as well as any ancient language of some long-gone civilization (Sumerian, Egyptian, etc.). A typical pseudolinguistic language family claim will show a list of similar words with similar meanings in both languages. As the Zompist has already eloquently torn apart such claims,[1] there is no need for debunking them here.
One example of the 'other way around' can be found in the book Hebrew is Greek by Joseph Yahuda, who claimed that he was able to break a cypher that enabled him to prove that ancient Hebrew and ancient Greek are the same language, and that "the Hebrews were of Hellenic descent, and that the Arabs were of Hittite (Scythian) origin; that they were both intimately related to the Greeks by religion and custom…"[2] This is despite the consensus view among linguists that Greek and Hittite are in the Indo-European language family,[3] and that Hebrew and Arabic are in the Afroasiatic language family.[4]
It's popular for nationalists across much of Eurasia to claim their country or region was the Proto-Indo-European homeland.[5]
If mixed with an unhealthy dose of religious whackery, you might find pseudolinguists looking for an "Adamic language", as allegedly spoken by all of mankind before the abandonment of the Tower of Babel. In the Middle Ages, perceptive scholars identified Basque as the Adamic language, as it was one of the few known languages clearly not related to any other language. (The search for Adamic languages should not be confused with the Proto-Human language theory, which is a legitimate - though controversial - hypothesis of all languages in the world having one origin.)
Religion often provides the basis for a lot of pseudolinguistics:
“” Glottochronology must be about the only scientific technique where the accuracy of
one’s results is enhanced by the removal rather than the augmentation of data!
|
—Douglas Adams and J.P Mallory[8]:95 |
Glottochronology, also called lexicostatistics, is a method of estimating the date of splitting between two languages. It involves the comparison of several languages' basic vocabulary and then deriving a date of diversion. In some ways, glottochronology can be compared to genetic analyses or carbon dating. Glottochronology was invented by Morris Swadesh, a historical linguist in the 1950s.[9]:427-8
Glottochronology rests on the following assumptions:[9]:423, 426-7
Unfortunately, glottochronology's assumptions are flawed:
Mass comparison, like the comparative method, assembles a list of proposed cognates between a large number of languages. Unlike the comparative method, it relies only on superficial similarities between languages and ignores the possibility of coincidences or borrowings. This makes results derived from it more error-prone, and connections between languages derived from it less trustworthy. [10]:168-172, 202-3 This method has been used by Joseph Greenberg, John Bengtson and Merritt Ruhlen et al. to argue for their far-reaching macrofamilies and for proto-world.
Some people make proposals for language relations based on non-linguistic evidence, like genetics. These proposals are invalid for several reasons:[10]:206
Macrofamilies are proposals to link large numbers of language families (and sometimes isolates).
Proto-World, or Proto-Human is the hypothetical ancestor of all languages today. While the idea that all living human languages go back to a common ancestor has merit, claims to have reconstructed what it looked like do not. Reconstructions of Proto-World typically use the following methodology:[10]:368
A. Ignore vowels entirely: Any vowel matches any other vowel
B. For consonants, roughly similar places of articulation suffices to match cognates [though non-initial consonants are sometimes allowed drastic differences]. Minor place changes are acceptable: velars match uvulars, palatals, etc. Other features play no role whatsoever, so that oral stops correspond to nasals, etc.
C. Any differences in place which parallel widely attested sound changes such as lenition are acceptable, so that any consonant can be reflected by [h]
D. In semantics, any narrowing or any metaphorical extension is acceptable without further justification (such as cultural or historical arguments), so that 'dog' corresponds to 'fox, lynx, deer' etc. and 'arm' to 'elbow/hand, fingernail, foot, armpit, shoulder/arm,' and so forth.
This methodology creates the potential for numerous false cognates. *kuna "woman", one of the most commonly cited examples of a supposed Proto-World word, exemplifies these problems:[10]:369
Lyle Campbell and William Poser found that numerous Spanish words with known etymologies and histories fit the patterns established by *kuna.[10]:370
Reconstructions of Proto-World face one major problem: After 6000 years, two languages are expected to share only 7% of their words. After 14000 years, nearly all of a languages basic vocabulary can be expected to change. This would make it very difficult to find true cognates even in situations where it is known that two languages are related (such as Hindi and English).[10] :380-6 At the time depths Proto-World is presumed to have existed, there is more than enough time for this to have happened several times.
Many nationalists like to claim that the language they speak was Proto-World. Some of the languages that been claimed to be Proto-World include Sanskrit, Hebrew, Gaelic,[14] Tamil[15], Guarani[note 6] Romanian, and Dutch.[17] The Sun Language Theory, claims that language was invented by the Turks as a way to convert ritual blathering into a means of meaningful communication. This "theory" was promoted by the Turkish government under Atatürk in the 1930s.
A particularly stupid example is Valery Chudinov.[18] He concluded that Russians are more ancient than most civilizations by "noticing" monosyllabic Russian words enscribed on ancient artifacts, the ocean floor, the sun's surface and drying plaster.
Occasionally, a lost continent like Mu, Lemuria, or Atlantis is invoked to explain how a language family spread.[19] Tamil has at one point been claimed to have spoken on the lost continent of Lemuria.[15]
Some people like to make connections between languages. These attempts are protoscience at best. Some examples include:
Conversely, some people like to claim that their language isn't related to another:
Undeciphered writing systems like Linear A from Crete or Rongorongo attract people who claim to have deciphered them but invariably have not.[29]
The Voynich Manuscript also attracts people who claim to have deciphered it, but invariably, they have not.[30]
Weak interpretations of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis (the concept that language has an effect on how we interpret the world) are considered to be a part of legitimate linguistics. However, many linguists now regard the original proposition of the hypothesis, and stronger interpretations (suggesting that language defines and constrains our thoughts) as pseudolinguistics. (Modern linguistics regards the thought-control through language in Nineteen Eighty-Four as impossible.) Related to this are linguistic myths such as the claim that the Inuit have hundreds of words for snow, supposedly reflecting their deep interest in the subject.[31]
Neurolinguistic programming also incorporates some pseudo-linguistic claims.
xenoglossy, the idea that an individual can spontaneously learn a language, is nonsense.
Some claims[32] used by proponents of international auxiliary languages (such as Esperanto) can be classified as pseudolinguistics.
Some people think that ancient astronauts taught us writing or that dead languages such as Egyptian are in fact descended from alien tongues. Aliens have in all likelihood never visited Earth for much the same reasons that we have not visited them, and if they did, it is highly unlikely their languages would be pronounceable by humans. Think Chewbacca.
On a related note, essentially anything specific about extraterrestrial languages, i.e. astrolinguistics, is pseudolinguistics since it's all protoscience and speculation in reality.
Then there's this guy:
Pseudolinguistics has been used a tool to discriminate. Until a few decades ago, the French claimed the regional languages of France were dialects of French[33] in the Vergonha in an effort to discredit them and weed them out from daily use. The French were mostly successful in that effort, as the minority languages (Occitan, Arpitan, Gallo, etc.) are almost extinct today and are kept going only by a special effort of their respective provinces. African American Vernacular English has also been discriminated against.
Real linguistics is about understanding how real people use language, and the chief reference is the language as it is really spoken. In contrast, prescriptivists want to tell people how language should be used, based not on real-life examples but on a variety of bizarre and illogical arguments, including historical arguments, comparison with other languages (usually Latin for English-language prescriptivists), and other logical fallacies. Oxford Bibliographies Online says of linguistic prescriptivism: "this ideology and its practices are now usually ascribed to nonlinguists or nonacademic linguists, whereas modern academic linguists ... restrict themselves to the study and description of the structure of language and its natural use."[34]
One common example is the so-called ban on splitting infinitives in English, such as in the phrase "to boldly go" which is considered bad writing by some. These have existed in English at least since the 14th century, but have been criticised by pedants since the Victorian age. Some people say this is based on a comparison with Latin because there is no such thing as a split infinitive in Latin: an infinitive in Latin is a single word, while in English it is two words ("to go"), so it is impossible to split an infinitive in Latin but easy in English. On the other hand, others point out that "go", not "to go", is actually the infinitive in English, and therefore suggest that there's no logic behind the prohibition: most of the infinitive police think it's uneducated or just don't like it.[35][36]
The only tangible explanation for everything that can be seen in EDAL in that respect, is that the Altaic hypothesis at least in its Moscow version became a set of beliefs highly reminiscent of a religion. However, religion and science cannot coexist, because the first is based on faith, while the second seeks the explanation of the facts that are in need of explanation. EDAL does not explain these facts, it simply creates the "evidence" for the pre-existing belief that is, of course, non-evidence.
Vladislav Markovich ("Slava") Illich-Svitych (1934–1966), henceforth IS, is highly revered by many who work with Nostratic, particularly by scholars from the ex-Soviet Union. IS is largely credited for launching the Nostratic hypothesis and for assembling much of the evidence upon which it is based. While the reverence is commendable, at times there appears to be almost a Slava cult, that is, an emotional attachment to all that IS wrote such that any attempt to move beyond it or to refine it — and heaven forbid to criticize it —, is simply rejected out of hand by those who share these sentiments (cf. Bulatova 1989, Dybo 1989a, Manaster Ramer 1993:228–231). This is the "style and sociology that always seem to loom large in any discussions of the Nostratic hypothesis", "the excessive veneration of Illich-Svitych’s memory and the defensiveness about just about every aspect of his theory" about which Manaster Ramer (1993:228, 230) wrote. For example, one suspects there is something of the Slava cult lying behind the very critical reactions from various Nostraticists towards Bomhard’s (1984, 1986, 1990, cf. also Bomhard & Kerns 1994) alternative view of Nostratic (see Kaiser & Shevoroshkin 1987, Markey & Shevoroshkin 1986:xxxii–xxxiv). The very strongly worded polemic from the very dedicated among the former Soviet inner circle of Nostraticists has often been observed (cf. Kaye 1989:223, Manaster Ramer 1993).
Campbell later describes an incident where a Nostraticist uses an obsolete reconstruction of Proto-Finno-Ugric, because Illich-Svitych used it:[13]:120-1
I suspect that there is something of the "Slava" cult lying behind Dybo’s remarks. The thinking would appear to go something like the following. Since IS used these earlier Uralic sources, others not yet being available to him, and since they are for this reason deeply interwoven into IS’s Nostratic reconstructions, and since these are held by the very dedicated to be absolutely correct, then the Uralic forms upon which IS relied must be valid by implication, given the asserted correctness of IS’s Nostratic achievement.